Tài liệu hạn chế xem trước, để xem đầy đủ mời bạn chọn Tải xuống
1
/ 85 trang
THÔNG TIN TÀI LIỆU
Thông tin cơ bản
Định dạng
Số trang
85
Dung lượng
435,68 KB
Nội dung
408 PART V. SELF-BASED APPROACHES terminants of favorable and unfavorable changes in self-esteem. Paper presented at the meeting of the Midwestern Psychological As- sociation, Chicago. Geary, D. G. (1998). Male, female. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Grossman, B. L. (1991). Children of interfaith marriage. Dissertation Abstracts International, 51 (5-B), 2621. Guttman, J., & Mills, M. K. (1982). Fashion life style, self-concept, shopping orientation, and store patronage: An integrative analysis. Journal of Retailing, 58, 64–86. Hornsey, M. J., & Hogg, M. A. (2000). Subgroup relations: A comparison of mutual intergroup differentiation and common ingroup identity models of prejudice reduction. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 242–256. James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology (Vol. 1). New York: Henry Holt. Janis, I. L. (1982). Groupthink (2nd ed.). Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Jetten, J., Spears, R., & Manstead, A. S. R. (1998). Defining dimensions of distinctiveness: Group variability makes a difference to differentiation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1481–1492. Kernis, M. H. (1984). Need for uniqueness, self- schemas, and thoughts as moderators of the false-consensus effect. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 20, 350–362. Kim, H., & Markus, H. R. (1999). Deviance or uniqueness, harmony or conformity? A cultural analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- chology, 77, 785–800. Lemaine, G. (1974). Social differentiation and so- cial originality. European Journal of Social Psy- chology, 4, 17–52. Lynn, M. (1987). The effects of scarcity on per- ceived value: Investigations of commodity the- ory. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Ohio State University, Columbus. Lynn, M. (1989). Scarcity effects on value: Medi- ated by assumed expensiveness? Journal of Eco- nomic Psychology, 10, 257–274. Lynn, M. (1991). Scarcity effects on value: A quantitative review of the commodity theory literature. Psychology and Marketing, 8, 43– 57. *Lynn, M., & Harris, J. (1997a). The desire for unique consumer products: A new individual differences scale. Psychology and Marketing, 14, 601–616. *Lynn, M., & Harris, J. (1997b). Individual differ- ences in the pursuit of self-uniqueness through consumption. Journal of Applied Social Psy- chology, 27, 1861–1883. Markus, H., & Kunda, Z. (1986). Stability and malleability of the self-concept. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 858– 866. Maslach, C. (1974). Social and personal bases of individuation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 29, 411–425. Maslach, C., Stapp, J., & Santee, R. T. (1985). In- dividuation: Conceptual analysis and assess- ment. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- chology, 49, 729–738. McCaul, K. D. (1978). Symptom information con- sensus and emotional responses to stress. Un- published doctoral dissertation, University of Kansas, Lawrence. Miller, D. T., & McFarland, C. (1987). Pluralistic ignorance: When similarity is interpreted as dis- similarity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 298–305. Miller, D. T., Turnbull, W., & McFarland, C. (1988). Particularistic and universalistic evalua- tion in the social comparison process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 908– 917. Nisbett, R. E., Borgida, E., Crandall, R., & Reed, H. (1976). Popular induction: Information is not always informative. In J. S. Carroll & J. W. Payne (Eds.), Cognition and social behavior (pp. 113–134). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Okamoto, K. (1983). Effects of excessive similarity feedback on subsequent mood, pursuit of differ- ence, and preference for novelty or scarcity. Jap- anese Psychological Research, 25, 69–77. Omens, A. E. (1977). The presentation of unique- ness behaviors as a function of need for unique- ness, audience, and sex. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Kansas, Lawrence. Powell, F. A. (1974). The perception of self- uniqueness as a determinant of message choice and valuation. Speech Monographs, 41, 163– 168. Robertson, T. S. (1971). Innovative behavior and communication. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. Rogers, E. M. (1983). Diffusion of innovations (3rd ed.). New York: Free Press. Schachter, S. (1959). The psychology of affiliation. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Schroeder, J. E. (1990). Psychological foundations of consumer preferences: The role of affiliation, conformity, individuation, and uniqueness. Un- published doctoral dissertation, University of California at Berkeley. Schroeder, J. E., & Dugal, S. S. (1995). Psycholog- ical correlates of the materialism construct. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 10, 243–253. CHAPTER 28. UNIQUENESS SEEKING 409 Simmel, G. (1957). Fashion. American Journal of Sociology, 62, 541–558. (Original work pub- lished 1904) Simon, B., & Hamilton, D. L. (1994). Self- stereotyping and social context: The effects of relative in-group size and in-group status. Jour- nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 699–711. Simons, T., Pelled, L. H., & Smith, K. A. (1999). Making use of difference: Diversity, debate and decision comprehensiveness in top management teams. Academy of Management Journal, 42, 662–673. Simonson, I., & Nowlis, S. M. (2000). The role of explanations and need for uniqueness in con- sumer decision making: Unconventional choices based on reasons. Journal of Consumer Re- search, 27, 49–68. Snyder, C. R. (1992). Product scarcity by need for uniqueness interaction: A consumer catch-22 carousel? Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 13, 9–24. Snyder, C. R. (1993). Hope for the journey. In A. Turnbull, J. M. Patterson, S. K. Behr, D. L. Mur- phy, J. G. Marquis, & M. J. Blue-Banning (Eds.), Cognitive coping, families, and disability: Par- ticipatory research in action (pp. 271–286). Bal- timore: Brookes. Snyder, C. R. (1994). The psychology of hope: You can get there from here. New York: Free Press. Snyder, C. R., & Batson, C. D. (1974). The bal- anced interpersonal perception of differences and similarities: A model of psychological dis- tance. Paper presented at the meeting of the Western Psychological Association, San Fran- cisco. Snyder, C. R., & Endelman, J. R. (1980). Effects of degree of interpersonal similarity on physical distance and self-reported attraction: A compar- ison of uniqueness and reinforcement theory predictions. Journal of Personality, 47, 492–505. Snyder, C. R., & Feldman, D. B. (2000). Hope for the many: An empowering social agenda. In C. R. Snyder (Ed.), Handbook of hope: Theory, measures, and applications (pp. 389–409). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Snyder, C. R., Feldman, D. B., Taylor, D. B., Schroeder, L. L., & Adams, III. V. (2000). The roles of hopeful thinking in preventing prob- lems and promoting strengths. Applied and Pre- ventive Psychology, 15, 262–295. *Snyder, C. R., & Fromkin, H. L. (1977). Abnor- mality as a positive characteristic: The develop- ment and validation of a scale measuring need for uniqueness. Journal of Abnormal Psychol- ogy, 86, 518–527. *Snyder, C. R., & Fromkin, H. L. (1980). Unique- ness: The human pursuit of difference. New York: Plenum. Snyder, C. R., & Shenkel, R. J. (1976). Effects of “favorability,” modality, and relevance upon ac- ceptance of general personality interpretations prior to and after receiving diagnostic feedback. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 32, 258–265. Snyder, C. R., Shenkel, R. J., & Lowery, C. R. (1977). Acceptance of personality interpreta- tions: The “Barnum Effect” and beyond. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 45, 104– 114. Sulloway, F. J. (1996). Born to rebel. New York: Pantheon. Swann, W. B. (1983). Self-verification: Bringing social reality into harmony with the self. In J. Suls & A. G. Greenwald (Eds.), Psychological perspectives on the self (Vol. 2, pp. 33–66). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Tepper, K. (1994). Need for uniqueness: An indi- vidual difference factor affecting nonconformity in consumer response. In C. W. Park & D. C. Smith (Eds.), Marketing theory and applica- tions: Proceedings of the 1994 AMA Winter Ed- ucators’ Conference. Chicago: American Mar- keting Association. Tepper, K. (1996a). Estimating model fit when data are not multivariate normal: An assessment of the generalizability of the consumers’ need for uniqueness scale. In C. Droge & R. Calantone (Eds.), AMA Educators’ Proceedings (Vol. 7, pp. 29–36). Chicago: American Marketing As- sociation. Tepper, K. (1996b). Understanding consumers’ counterconformity behavior: A critical appraisal of trait measures employed in nonconformity research. In C. Droge & R. Calantone (Eds.), AMA Educators’ Proceedings (Vol. 7, pp. 252– 257). Chicago: American Marketing Association. Tepper, K., Bearden, W. O., & Hunter, G. (in press). Consumers’ need for uniqueness: Scale development and validation. Journal of Con- sumer Research, Lexington. Tepper, K., & Hoyle, R. H. (1996). Latent variable models of need for uniqueness. Multivariate Be- havioral Research, 31, 467–494. Tepper, K., & McKenzie, K. (2001). The long-term predictive validity of the consumers’ need for uniqueness scale. Journal of Consumer Psy- chology, 10, 171–193. Tesser, A. (1988). Toward a self-evaluation main- tenance model of social behavior. In L. Berko- witz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psy- chology (Vol. 21, pp. 181–227). San Diego, CA: Academic Press 410 PART V. SELF-BASED APPROACHES Weir, H. B. (1971). Deprivation of the need for uniqueness and some variables moderating its effects. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Uni- versity of Georgia, Athens. Wilson, T. D., & Schooler, J. W. (1991). Think- ing too much: Introspection can reduce the qual- ity of preferences and decisions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 181– 192. Zweigenhaft, R. L. (1981). Unusual names and uniqueness. Journal of Social Psychology, 114, 297–298. 411 29 Humility June Price Tangney Although humility is commonly equated with a sense of unworthiness and low self-regard, true humility is a rich, multifaceted construct that is characterized by an accurate assessment of one’s characteristics, an ability to acknowl- edge limitations, and a “forgetting of the self.” In this chapter, I describe current conceptions of humility, discuss the challenges in its measure- ment, and review the scant empirical work ad- dressing it directly and indirectly. I also will discuss briefly interventions for enhancing humility. History of the Psychology of Humility: Still at the Point of Humble Beginnings Scientific study of humility is still in its infancy. A review of the empirical literature from the last 20 years yields only a handful of research studies with any consideration of this long-revered construct. Furthermore, in virtually every case where humility is ad- dressed, it has been tangential to the main re- search focus. Why has humility been neglected so long? Two factors come readily to mind. First, the concept of humility is linked to values and re- ligion in many people’s minds. As a field, for many years, mainstream psychology steered clear of such value-laden topics as religion, vir- tue, and (with the exception of Kohlberg’s work on forms of moral thinking) morality. In their zeal to establish psychology as a bona fide science, psychological scientists embraced no- tions of objectivity and fact. Indeed, it is worth noting that the virtues as a group have been relatively neglected in psychology. Until very recently, wisdom, gratitude, and forgiveness, for example, all represented “black holes” in the literature based on a century of psycholog- ical science. A second factor undoubtedly contributing to the neglect of humility is the lack of a well- established measure of this construct. If you can’t measure it, you can’t study it. Psychology and the sciences in general are chock full of ex- amples of how an advance in measurement can lead to a dramatic expansion in empirical re- search. For example, after years of neglect, the scientific study of shame virtually exploded in the early 1990s—shortly after the development of several psychometrically sound, easily ad- ministered measures of individual differences in proneness to shame (Harder & Lewis, 1987; Hoblitzelle, 1987; Tangney, 1990). As discussed in greater detail later, measurement remains a significant challenge in the area of humility. 412 PART V. SELF-BASED APPROACHES Contrasting Conceptions of Humility Another challenge facing psychological scien- tists interested in humility centers on the vary- ing definitions of the construct. For many, hu- mility simply means holding oneself in low regard. For example, in the Oxford English Dictionary (1998), humility is defined as “the quality of being humble or having a lowly opin- ion of oneself; meekness, lowliness, humble- ness: the opposite of pride or haughtiness.” In other dictionaries, humility is defined largely as a state of being “humble,” which in turn is de- fined, for example, by Funk & Wagnalls Stan- dard College Dictionary (1963) as “lowly in kind, state, condition, etc.; of little worth; un- important; common Lowly in feeling; lack- ing self-esteem; having a sense of insignificance, unworthiness, dependence, or sinfulness; meek; penitent” (p. 653). From this “low self-esteem” perspective, humility certainly does not stand out as one of the more attractive virtues. For example, most of us would have difficulty ap- preciating a friend’s efforts to strengthen our character by “humbling” us (e.g., making us lower in state or condition, reducing possessions or esteem, abasing us). The “low self-esteem” conception of humility is prevalent not only in dictionaries but also in the psychological literature (e.g., Klein, 1992; Knight & Nadel, 1986; Langston & Cantor, 1988; Weiss & Knight, 1980), as well as in com- mon parlance. Nonetheless, it is clear that when “experts” (e.g., philosophers, theologians, soci- ologists, psychologists, and other “wise” per- sons) delve into the broader significance of hu- mility, they have a different—and much richer—notion of this construct. Emmons (1998) clearly articulated this alter- native view of humility by stating: Although humility is often equated in peo- ple’s minds with low self-regard and tends to activate images of a stooped-shouldered, self-deprecating, weak-willed soul only too willing to yield to the wishes of others, in reality humility is the antithesis of this car- icature. To be humble is not to have a low opinion of oneself, it is to have an accurate opinion of oneself. It is the ability to keep one’s talents and accomplishments in per- spective (Richards, 1992), to have a sense of self-acceptance, an understanding of one’s imperfections, and to be free from arrogance and low self-esteem (Clark, 1992) (p. 33). Templeton (1997) presents a similar conceptu- alization of humility: Humility is not self-deprecation. To believe that you have no worth, or were created somehow flawed or incompetent, can be foolish. Humility represents wisdom. It is knowing you were created with special tal- ents and abilities to share with the world; but it can also be an understanding that you are one of many souls created by God, and each has an important role to play in life. Humility is knowing you are smart, but not all-knowing. It is accepting that you have personal power, but are not omnipotent Inherent in humility resides an open and receptive mind itleaves us more open to learn from others and refrains from seeing issues and people only in blacks and whites. The opposite of humility is arrogance—the belief that we are wiser or better than oth- ers. Arrogance promotes separation rather than community. It looms like a brick wall between us and those from whom we could learn. (pp. 162–163) For many, there is a religious dimension to humility—the recognition that “God infinitely exceeds anything anyone has ever said of Him, and that He is infinitely beyond human com- prehension and understanding” (Templeton, 1997, p. 30; see also Schimmel, 1997). Here, too, the emphasis is not on human sinfulness, un- worthiness, and inadequacy but rather on the notion of a higher, greater power and the im- plication that, although we may have consider- able wisdom and knowledge, there always are limits to our perspective. Humility carries with it an open-mindedness, a willingness to admit mistakes and seek advice, and a desire to learn (Hwang, 1982; Templeton, 1997). Also inherent in the state of humility is a relative lack of self-focus or self-preoccupation. Templeton (1997) refers to a process of becom- ing “unselved,” which goes hand in hand with the recognition of one’s place in the world. A person who has gained a sense of humility is no longer phenomenologically at the center of his or her world. The focus is on the larger com- munity, of which he or she is one part. From this perspective, the excessively self-deprecating person can be seen, in some important respects, as lacking humility. Consider the person who repeatedly protests, “Oh, I’m not really very good in art. I never did very well in art class at CHAPTER 29. HUMILITY 413 school. Oh, this little painting that I did really is nothing. I just whipped it together last night. It (my painting) is really nothing.” Such appar- ently humble protests betray a marked self- focus. The person remains at the center of at- tention, with the self as the focus of con- sideration and evaluation. In relinquishing the very human tendency toward an egocentric focus, persons with hu- mility become ever more open to recognizing the abilities, potential, worth, and importance of others. One important consequence of becoming “unselved” is that we no longer have the need to enhance and defend an all-important self at the expense of our evaluation of others (Hall- ing, Kunz, & Rowe, 1994). Our attention shifts outward, and our eyes are opened to the beauty and potential in those around us. As Means, Wilson, Sturm, Biron, and Bach (1990) ob- served, humility “is an increase in the valuation of others and not a decrease in the valuation of oneself” (p. 214). Myers (1979) effectively cap- tured these latter two elements of humility, stating: The true end of humility is not self- contempt Toparaphrase C. S. Lewis, humility does not consist in handsome people trying to believe they are ugly, and clever people trying to believe they are fools True humility is more like self- forgetfulness Itleaves people free to es- teem their special talents and, with the same honesty, to esteem their neighbor’s. Both the neighbor’s talents and one’s own are recognized as gifts and, like one’s height, are not fit subjects for either inordinate pride or self-deprecation. (p. 38) In the theological, philosophical, and psycho- logical literatures, therefore, humility is por- trayed as a rich, multifaceted construct, in sharp contrast to dictionary definitions that emphasize a sense of unworthiness and low self-regard. Specifically, the key elements of humility seem to include: • an accurate assessment of one’s abilities and achievements (not low self-esteem, self- deprecation) • an ability to acknowledge one’s mistakes, imperfections, gaps in knowledge, and limi- tations (often vis-a`-vis a “higher power”) • openness to new ideas, contradictory infor- mation, and advice • keeping one’s abilities and accomplishments— one’s place in the world—in perspective (e.g., seeing oneself as just one person in the larger scheme of things) • a relatively low self-focus, a “forgetting of the self,” while recognizing that one is but part of the larger universe • an appreciation of the value of all things, as well as the many different ways that people and things can contribute to our world What Humility Is Not Humility is a rich psychological construct that is related to, but conceptually distinct from, fa- miliar constructs such as narcissism, modesty, and self-esteem. Clearly, from the foregoing discussion, humility is not low self-esteem (Ryan, 1983), nor is it an underestimation of one’s abilities, accomplishments, or worth. Fur- thermore, as explained subsequently, humility is related to, but distinct from, modesty and narcissism. The concept of modesty focuses primarily on a moderate estimate of personal merits or achievements. As such, modesty does not cap- ture other key aspects of humility such as a “forgetting of the self” and an appreciation of the variety of ways in which others can be “worthy.” Rather, use of the term “modesty” often extends into issues of propriety in behav- ior and dress, where the notion of humility is less relevant. Thus, modesty is both too narrow, missing fundamental components of humility, and too broad, relating also to bodily exposure and other dimensions of propriety. One might view modesty—in the sense of an accurate, unexaggerated estimation of one’s strengths— as a component of humility. But it does not tell the whole story. The construct of narcissism is perhaps most closely related to humility. People who are nar- cissistic clearly lack humility. It is not clear, however, that an absence of narcissism can be equated with the presence of humility. In con- ceptualizing narcissism, social psychologists tend to focus on grandiosity, an exaggerated sense of self-importance, and an overestimation of one’s abilities. But there’s much more to the clinical conceptions of narcissism. Clinical the- orists, drawing on a long history of “object re- lations,” typically use the term narcissism to refer to a distinctly pathological form of self- focus and fluctuating self-regard, which stems from fundamental defects in the self system 414 PART V. SELF-BASED APPROACHES (e.g., Kohut, 1971). When clinicians refer to a person with narcissism, they generally have in mind a seriously disturbed individual who ex- hibits pervasive adjustment difficulties that go hand in hand with a DSM-IV (American Psy- chiatric Association, 1994) diagnosis of person- ality disorder. This is not simply an over- confident, conceited dolt, but rather someone with a damaged sense of self. Attempts to shore up the self with unrealistic fantasies of grandi- osity inevitably alternate with a grinding sense of emptiness and self-loathing. Other hallmarks of narcissism include a pervasive self-focus and a corresponding inability to focus on and em- pathize with others. Narcissistic individuals clearly lack many of the essential components of humility. But it is not clear that people who score low on a mea- sure of narcissism necessarily embody humility. People low on narcissism may or may not make accurate assessments of their abilities and achievements. For example, low-self-esteem, self-deprecating individuals are neither narcis- sistic nor paragons of humility. Similarly, peo- ple without narcissistic tendencies may or may not have the wisdom to keep their places in the world in perspective (e.g., seeing themselves as one person in the larger scheme of things). They may or may not have a deep appreciation for the unique gifts and talents of others. In defining complex constructs such as hu- mility, as well as in developing measurement instruments, it is important to specify how the focal construct differs from other related but distinct concepts. As underscored by Campbell and Fiske (1959), discriminant validity is a crit- ical component of measurement validation. It is important to know not only that a measure cor- relates well (positively or negatively) with (measures of) other relevant constructs in a the- oretically meaningful way. It is also important to demonstrate that the measure does not cor- relate too highly with (or behave identically to) established measures of some other construct. Measures that are “confounded” by items tapping other nonfocal constructs not only pre- sent conceptual ambiguity but also impede sci- ence by blurring the boundaries between con- structs, inadvertently precluding the possibility of studying functional relations among them. For example, in the case of forgiveness, it is im- possible to examine meaningfully the functional relationship between empathy and forgiveness if one uses a forgiveness measure that includes items tapping empathy. In short, it is important to decide where to draw the conceptual line. Measurement of Humility: Two Levels of Measurement, Two Levels of Questions Halling et al. (1994) observed that doing re- search on humility is humbling. Quite possibly, the quest for a reliable and valid measure of humility is the most humbling aspect of re- search on this topic. By its very nature, the con- struct of humility poses some special challenges in the area of measurement. As a consequence, psychological scientists have yet to develop a well-validated tool for assessing humility. This is a glaring gap in the literature, because with- out a solid assessment method, the science pretty much comes to a halt. It is also worth noting that psychologists are most likely to de- velop strong, meaningful measures when those measures are informed by theory. Although we have some clear definitions of humility, com- prehensive theories or models need to be de- veloped and refined, which in turn would form a solid foundation for assessment. Theoretically, humility could be assessed at two distinct levels—at the level of states and at the level of dispositions.Adispositional assess- ment would focus on stable, individual differ- ences in humility. In this context, humility would be viewed as a component of one’s per- sonality, as a relatively enduring disposition that a person brings to many different kinds of situations. In contrast, a state measure would focus on feelings or experiences of humility “in the moment.” Personality and individual differ- ences aside, most of us have humility in some situations but not in others. Regarding dispositional humility, a few op- tions presently are available to researchers, but each has significant drawbacks. In several earlier studies, humility has been operationalized as low self-esteem (e.g., Weiss & Knight, 1980), but this clearly is inconsistent with broader con- ceptualizations of humility. In fact, theoreti- cally, scores on self-esteem measures such as the Rosenberg (1965) and Janis and Fields (1956) scales should be positively correlated with (although not identical to) individual dif- ferences in humility. Consider the types of items included on the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (rated on a scale of 1 to 5, from “always false” to “always true”): “I feel that I’m a per- CHAPTER 29. HUMILITY 415 son of worth, at least on an equal plane with others” and “I feel I have a number of good qualities.” The person with a true sense of hu- mility would be expected to endorse such items positively, not negatively. Taking a different approach, Farh, Dobbins, and Cheng (1991) and Yu and Murphy (1993) operationalized workers’ “modesty” by compar- ing self-ratings to ratings made of them by knowledgeable others (e.g., supervisors and co- workers). Those who rated themselves lower than their supervisors were viewed as showing a “modesty bias.” Here, too, there are some conceptual ambiguities with such “self versus knowledgeable other” comparisons. Given that humility theoretically entails an accurate as- sessment of one’s abilities, one could argue that high humility should be indexed by high levels of agreement between self and other, not self- deprecating discrepancies. Emmons (personal communication, Decem- ber 4, 1998) attempted to develop a self-report measure of humility. Using a forced-choice for- mat to circumvent social desirability biases, Em- mons developed an array of theoretically de- rived items tapping the diverse components of humility described previously. The measure is well crafted in design and content. Unfortu- nately, Emmons’s initial analyses of the mea- sure’s internal reliability were disappointing, and he is now rather skeptical that this con- struct can be adequately assessed via self-report. With regard to experiences of humility “in the moment,” currently there is no established self-report measure of state humility. But Ex- line, Bushman, Faber, and Phillips (2000) re- cently developed a technique for experimentally inducing a sense of humility by asking people to write about “a time when they felt humble or experienced a sense of humility” versus “a time when they felt important or had a sense of importance.” Based on results from an initial study, some complications arise in using this technique to prime humility. Specifically, peo- ple receiving humility instructions wrote two very different types of narratives. The majority of persons described instances in which they felt bad about themselves for doing something stu- pid or wrong. For this group, the instructions seemed to prime a sense of humiliation or shame rather than a sense of humility. A smaller subset of respondents described events that seemed more directly to the experience of humility—for example, situations that evoked a “forgetting of the self” or that caused respon- dents to see themselves in a broader context. Thus, in using the Exline et al. (2000) priming technique, it is important to distinguish be- tween stories involving humiliation versus hu- mility themes. In addition, some modifications to the instructions may be necessary in order to more consistently elicit stories of “true” hu- mility rather than shaming experiences. No doubt, psychologists will continue efforts to develop psychometrically sound measures of both state and dispostional humility in the years to come. It is worth noting that research- ers generally rely on self-report methods for as- sessing personality traits. In the case of humil- ity, however, there is a potentially serious catch. To the degree that a key component of humility is a “forgetting of the self,” self-reflection and self-report of one’s level of humility may be oxymoronic. What do we make of a person who views him- or herself as someone with “unu- sually high humility”? As Halling et al. (1994) point out, “One can reflect on one’s own ex- perience of fear, isolation, or self-rejection, but the attention during the experience of humility is directed toward others” (p. 121). Similarly, Singh (1967) observed that “true humility is freedom from all consciousness of self, which includes freedom from the consciousness of hu- mility. The truly humble man never knows that he is humble” (p. 4). There are good reasons for psychologists’ preference for self-report measures of person- ality traits. Traits such as humility are not eas- ily inferred from quick observation. Also, sys- tematic behavioral observational methods are cumbersome and time-consuming. So there is a strong preference for paper-and-pencil ques- tionnaires that require little time and training to administer and score. But humility may rep- resent a rare personality construct that is simply unamenable to direct self-report methods. Thus, the present bottom line is that the measurement of humility remains an unsolved challenge in psychology. Psychological and Social Implications of Humility: Relevant Empirical Research Researchers have yet to directly address the psychology of humility and develop a theory- based, reliable, and valid measure. Some in- sights can be gleaned, however, from related ar- 416 PART V. SELF-BASED APPROACHES eas of psychological research. In this section, I provide a brief review of relevant findings from related literatures. Basic research on the self and its operations suggests that humility may be a relatively rare human characteristic. The pervasiveness of “self-enhancement biases” is underscored in the social psychological literature (Baumeister, 1998; Greenwald, 1980). From this literature, we learn that the self is remarkably resourceful at accentuating the positive and deflecting the negative. For example, research consistently shows that people are inclined to take credit for “their” successes but blame other factors for “their” failures and transgressions (Baumeister, Stillwell, & Wotman, 1990; Snyder, Higgins, & Stucky, 1983; Zuckerman, 1979). As another example, people are more likely to notice, think about, and remember positive information about themselves, with negative information being “lost in the shuffle” (Mischel, Ebbesen, & Zeiss, 1976). Indeed based on this self- enhancement literature, one might infer that humility is quite antithetical with human na- ture. Nonetheless, people apparently can control the degree to which they self-enhance in re- sponse to situational demands. On this point, Tice, Butler, Muraven, and Stillwell (1995) demonstrated that people adjust their self- enhancement according to the nature of the so- cial setting, showing more modesty in the com- pany of friends than strangers. Whether with friends or strangers, some de- gree of humility may be beneficial. The benefits of modesty—especially “moderate” modesty— have been underscored in numerous studies (Baumeister & Ilko, 1995; Bond, Leung, & Wan, 1982; Forsyth, Berger, & Mitchell, 1981; Jones & Wortman, 1973; Robinson, Johnson, & Shields, 1995). People like and feel less threat- ened by others who are modest about their achievements, whereas boastful, arrogant be- havior often results in social disapproval. The benefits of modesty seem to extend beyond pos- itive evaluation in purely social contexts. In an- swer to the objection that “you can’t get ahead without tooting your own horn,” Wosinka, Da- bul, Whetstone-Dion, and Cialdini (1996) have provided some evidence that modesty can be at- tractive in work contexts, as well. Likewise, tendencies toward self-enhance- ment, grandiosity, and narcissism bode poorly for long-term adjustment, especially in the in- terpersonal realm (Ehrenberg, Hunter, & Elter- man, 1996; Means et al., 1990). Although much has been written about the benefits of various “positive illusions” (Brown, 1993; Taylor & Brown, 1988, 1994), researchers also have shown repeatedly that tendencies toward self- enhancement are problematic. Specifically, psy- chological maladjustment is associated with the degree to which people rate themselves more favorably than others rate them (Asendorpf & Ostendorf, 1998; Colvin, Block, & Funder, 1995). Joiner and Perez (2000) also found that people who are immodest (relative to how oth- ers rate them) are more inclined toward physical aggression than are their more modest peers. Along the same lines, researchers have shown that narcissistic individuals are sensitive to in- terpersonal slights, quick to anger, and less in- clined to forgive others (Exline & Baumeister, 2000; Exline, Campbell, Baumeister, Joiner, & Krueger, in press; Sandage, Worthington, Hight, & Berry, 1999; Tangney, Boone, Fee, & Reinsmith, 1999). From these findings, one might infer that a sense of humility inhibits an- ger and aggression and fosters forgiveness. In one of the few studies to explicitly address the psychology of humility, Exline et al. (2000) found results suggestive of a link between hu- mility and forgiveness. People who were suc- cessfully primed to experience humility (e.g., who wrote personal accounts of a non-self- deprecating humility experience) were slower to retaliate in response to provocation on a labo- ratory task. In contrast, individuals primed to feel morally superior judged another person’s transgression more harshly and as less forgiv- able. Humility not only implies an accurate as- sessment of oneself (neither unduly favorable nor unfavorable) but also entails a “forgetting of the self,” an outwardly directed orientation toward a world in which one is “just one part.” This process of becoming “unselved” may have significant psychological and physical benefits. Clinicians have long noted the links between excessive self-focus and a broad range of psy- chological symptoms, including anxiety, depres- sion, social phobias, and so on. As Baumeister (1991) argues, there are many advantages to “escaping the self,” not the least of which is a relief from the burden of self-preoccupation (Halling et. al., 1994) and the “Western” im- perative to defend the vulnerable self. Even in the area of physical health, researchers suggest [...]... Psychological Bulletin, 56, 81–105 Clark, A T (1992) Humility In D H Ludlow (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Mormonism (pp 66 3– 66 4) New York: Macmillan Colvin, C R., Block, J., & Funder, D C (1995) Overly positive self-evaluations and personality: Negative implications for mental health Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68 , 1152– 1 162 Ehrenberg, M F., Hunter, M A., & Elterman, M F (19 96) Shared parenting... hard Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 64 9 66 1 Means, J R., Wilson, G L., Sturm, C., Biron, J E., & Bach, P J (1990) Theory and practice: Humility as a psychotherapeutic formulation Counseling Psychology Quarterly, 3, 211– 215 Mischel, W., Ebbesen, E B., & Zeiss, A R (19 76) Determinants of selective memory about the CHAPTER 29 HUMILITY self Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, ... the life of humans in a state of nature (the absence of society) is solitary, poor, nasty, brutish, and short Hobbes ( 165 1/ 1 962 ) believed that this facet of human nature, that humankind is brutal, was held in check by self-interest and humankind’s universal fear of death Schopenhauer (1 969 ), two centuries later, agreed on the importance of self-interest as one of the three basic motives of human behavior... H (1 960 ) Plans and the structure of behavior New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston Mills, J., & Clark, M S (1982) Exchange and communal relationships Review of Personality and Social Psychology, 3, 121–144 Murray, S L., Holmes, J G., & Griffin, D W (19 96) The benefits of positive illusions: Idealization and the construction of satisfaction in close relationships Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, ... Fabrication and revision of personal history American Psychologist, 35, 60 3 61 8 Halling, S., Kunz, G., & Rowe, J O (1994) The contributions of dialogal psychology to phenomenological research Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 34, 109–131 Harder, D W., & Lewis, S J (1987) The assessment of shame and guilt In J N Butcher & C D Spielberger (Eds.), Advances in personality assessment (Vol 6, pp 89–114) Hillsdale,... self-ratings of performance: A test of the cultural relativity hypothesis Personnel Psychology, 46, 357– 363 Zuckerman, M (1979) Attribution of success and failure revisited: or The motivational bias is alive and well in attribution theory Journal of Personality, 47, 245–287 This page intentionally left blank VI Interpersonal Approaches This page intentionally left blank 30 Relationship Connection The Role of. .. and Montgomery (19 96) have identified four possible risks of disclosing: rejection by the listener, reduction of one’s autonomy and personal integrity, loss of control or self-efficacy, and the possibility of hurting or embarrassing the listener All of these risks affect willingness to open up to a new partner; an atmosphere of respect and acceptance reduces these risks A climate of acceptance not only... relationships Annual Review of Psychology, 45, 79–129 Downey, G., & Feldman, S I (19 96) Implications of rejection sensitivity for intimate relationships Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 1327–1343 Fehr, R (1988) Prototype analysis of the concepts of love and commitment Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 557–579 Gottman, J (1994) What predicts divorce? The relationship between... acknowledgement of external help in accounts of success Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 16, 191–209 Baumeister, R F., Stillwell, A., & Wotman, S R (1990) Victim and perpetrator accounts of in- 418 PART V SELF-BASED APPROACHES terpersonal conflict: Autobiographical narratives about anger Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 994–1005 Bond, M H., Leung, K., & Wan, K C (1982) The social impact of self-effacing... (1995) The psychology of intimacy New York: Guilford Reis, H T., & Patrick, B C (19 96) Attachment and intimacy: Component processes In E T Higgins & A Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp 523– 563 ) New York: Guilford Rusbult, C E., Zembrodt, I M., & Gunn, L K (1982) Exit, voice, loyalty, and neglect: Responses to dissatisfaction in romantic involvements Journal of Personality . Psy- chology, 27, 1 861 –1883. Markus, H., & Kunda, Z. (19 86) . Stability and malleability of the self-concept. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 858– 866 . Maslach, C. (1974) The effects of relative in-group size and in-group status. Jour- nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66 , 69 9–711. Simons, T., Pelled, L. H., & Smith, K. A. (1999). Making use of difference:. 56, 81–105. Clark, A. T. (1992). Humility. In D. H. Ludlow (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Mormonism (pp. 66 3– 66 4). New York: Macmillan. Colvin, C. R., Block, J., & Funder, D. C. (1995). Overly positive