Tài liệu hạn chế xem trước, để xem đầy đủ mời bạn chọn Tải xuống
1
/ 92 trang
THÔNG TIN TÀI LIỆU
Thông tin cơ bản
Định dạng
Số trang
92
Dung lượng
1,19 MB
Nội dung
ICA BASED EEG ENERGY SPECTRUM
FOR DETECTION OF NEGATIVE EMTION BY EEG
ZHAN LIANG
A THESIS SUBMITTED
FOR THE DEGREE OF MASTER OF SCIENCE
GRADUATE PROGRAMME IN BIOENGINEERING
NATIONAL UNIVERSITY OF SINGAPORE
2007
Acknowledgement
ACKNOWLEDGEMENT
First of all, I would like to express my sincere appreciation to my supervisor,
Professor Li Xiaoping for his gracious guidance, a global view of research, strong
encouragement and detailed recommendations throughout the course of this research.
His patience, encouragement and support always gave me great motivation and
confidence in conquering the difficulties encountered in the study. His kindness will
always be gratefully remembered.
I would also like to thank my co-supervisor Associate Professor E.P.V. Wilder-Smith,
from the Department of Medicine, for his advice and kind help to this research.
I am also thankful to my colleagues, Mr. Ng Wu Chun, Mr. Ning Ning, Mr. Shen
Kaiquan, Mr. Fan Jie, Ms. Shao Shiyun, Mr. Chia Shan Ming, Mr. Oon Liyang and
Mr. Chong Shau Poh for their kind help, support and encouragement to my work. The
warm and friendly environment they created in the lab made my study in NUS an
enjoyable and memorable experience. I am also grateful to Dr. Qian Xinbo and Dr.
Seet Hang Li for their kind support to my study and work.
I would like to express my sincere thanks to the National University of Singapore and
Graduate Programme in Bioengineering for providing me with this great opportunity
and resource to conduct this research work.
Finally, I wish to express my deep gratitude to my parents, my sister and my wife for
their endless love and support. This thesis is dedicated to my parents.
i
Table of Contents
TABLE of CONTENTS
ACKNOWLEDGEMENT...........................................................................................I
TABLE OF CONTENTS .......................................................................................... II
SUMMARY ...............................................................................................................IV
LIST OF FIGURES ..................................................................................................VI
LIST OF TABLES .................................................................................................VIII
1.
INTRODUCTION............................................................................................... 1
1.1.
1.2.
1.3.
2.
BACKGROUND ...................................................................................................................... 1
PROBLEM STATEMENTS ...................................................................................................... 3
RESEARCH OBJECTIVES ...................................................................................................... 5
LITERATURE REVIEW .................................................................................. 7
2.1
TRADITIONAL TECHNOLOGIES IN EMOTION DETECTION .................................................. 7
2.1.1
Facial Analysis technologies.......................................................................................... 7
2.1.2
Speech Recognition technologies................................................................................... 8
2.1.3
Tradition methods disadvantages................................................................................... 9
2.2
EEG-BASED EMOTION MEASUREMENT ............................................................................. 9
2.2.1
Event-Related Potentials (ERPs) ..................................................................................10
2.2.2
Cerebral Electricity Asymmetry.....................................................................................13
3.
ICA-BASED EEG ENERGY SPECTRUM.................................................... 18
3.1
3.2
3.2.1
3.3
3.3.1
3.3.2
3.3.3
3.3.4
3.4
4
BIOLOGICAL BASIS .............................................................................................................18
INDEPENDENT COMPONENT ANALYSIS (ICA)...................................................................19
ICA Algorithm ............................................................................................................20
SCALP EEG MAPPING ........................................................................................................23
Grid generation..............................................................................................................24
Interpolation ..................................................................................................................25
Equivalent contour calculation .....................................................................................25
Color bar scaling ...........................................................................................................26
ICA-BASED EEG ENERGY SPECTRUM ..............................................................................27
EXPERIMENTAL DESIGN............................................................................ 31
4.1
4.1.1
4.1.2
4.1.3
4.2
4.3
4.3.1
4.3.2
4.3.3
4.3.4
4.4
4.4.1
4.4.2
BIOLOGICAL BASIS OF EMOTION.......................................................................................31
Emotion Loop ................................................................................................................32
Function of limbic system .............................................................................................33
Key components of limbic system..................................................................................35
EEG ELECTRODE PLACEMENTS ........................................................................................38
EXPERIMENTAL PROTOCOL ...............................................................................................41
International Affective Picture System (IAPS).............................................................42
Electrical shocks ............................................................................................................43
Overview Protocol..........................................................................................................43
Detailed Protocol ...........................................................................................................45
EXPERIMENTAL MATERIALS .............................................................................................47
Experiment Participants................................................................................................47
EEG Machine ................................................................................................................48
ii
Table of Contents
4.5
SIGNAL PROCESSING METHODS ........................................................................................49
4.6
SUPPORT VECTOR MACHINE (SVM) VERIFICATION ........................................................52
4.6.1
SVM basic algorism.......................................................................................................53
4.6.2
Data Labeling ................................................................................................................57
4.6.3
Feature Extraction ........................................................................................................57
4.6.4
Training and testing SVM model ..................................................................................59
5
RESULTS AND DISCUSSIONS ..................................................................... 61
5.1
EFFECTIVENESS OF ICA-BASED EEG ENERGY SPECTRUM ..............................................61
5.1.1
Anterior Temporal Energy Spectrum in negative emotion states vs. neutral emotion
state
........................................................................................................................................61
5.1.2
Asymmetry of Prefrontal Energy Spectrum in negative emotion states vs. control
emotion state.................................................................................................................................64
5.1.3
Validation of Experiment design...................................................................................68
5.1.4
SVM Verification of the EEG data ...............................................................................71
6
CONCLUSIONS ............................................................................................... 73
6.1
CONCLUSIONS .....................................................................................................................73
6.1.1
ICA-based EEG Energy Spectrum has been proposed.................................................73
6.1.2
Negative emotions, especially anxiety, causes discernible differences in EEG data in
compared with neutral emotion and these differences are detectable using EEG.....................74
6.2
RECOMMENDATIONS FOR FUTURE WORK ........................................................................75
REFERENCES.......................................................................................................... 77
iii
Summary
SUMMARY
In recent years, there are increasing interests in emotion-measurement technologies
with the widespread hope that they will be invaluable in the safety, medical and
criminal investigation. In the literature, various efforts have been put in the emotion
measurement methods, including facial recognition, voice recognition, and
electrophysiological based measurements. Among them, Electroencephalogram (EEG)
might be one of the most predictive and reliable physiological indicators of emotion.
However, most previously published research findings on EEG changes in
relationship to emotion have found varying, even conflicting results, which could be
due to methodological limitation. It needs further research before we can eventually
come out with an EEG-based emotion monitor.
For detection of anxiety emotion by EEG measurement, an Independent Component
Analysis (ICA) based energy spectrum feature is presented. In this study, EEG
measurements on human subjects with and without anxiety emotion were conducted,
the measured data was decomposed using ICA into a number of independent
components, and all the independent components were loaded on an energy mapping
system that shows the locations of the independent components on the scalp. By
counting the number of independent components fall into both sides of the anterior
temporal, clear correlation between the number of independent components on both
sides of the anterior temporal and the status of anxiety emotion was observed. The
results from all the subjects tested showed that in both sides of the anterior temporal,
iv
Summary
the number of independent components for anxiety status was 50% to 100% higher
than that of emotion void status. The ability of this ICA-based method was verified
by SVM prediction accuracy. Prediction accuracy shows that there is a high
probability to develop subject-specific negative emotion monitoring system.
v
List of Figures
LIST OF FIGURES
Figure 1.1 James-Lange Theory ........................................................................... 2
Figure 1.2 Cannon-Bard Theory ........................................................................ 2
Figure 1.3 Schachter’s two-factor Theory ......................................................... 3
Figure 2.1 Facial emotion analyses.................................................................... 8
Figure 2.2 International 10-20 EEG standard electrode positions................... 11
Figure 2.3 Using ERPs to differentiate negative/positive emotions ................ 12
Figure 2.4 Emotion detection using brain asymmetry ..................................... 14
Figure 3.1 Some Brain Activities..................................................................... 18
Figure 3.2 Illustration of Independent Component Analysis........................... 20
Figure 3.3 Independent Component Analysis.................................................. 23
Figure 3.4 Grid generation............................................................................... 24
Figure 3.5 Illustration of linear interpolation................................................... 25
Figure 3.6 Illustration of equivalent contour ................................................... 25
Figure 3.7 Color scaling algorism.................................................................... 26
Figure 3.8 Example of Scalp EEG map ........................................................... 26
Figure 3.9 Scalp EEG mapping for the ICA results......................................... 27
Figure 3.10 Four direction view of 3D scalp EEG mapping for the ICA result
..................................................................................................................... 28
Figure 3.11 classification of 3D scalp EEG mapping for the ICA results.......... 28
Figure 4.1 Flowchart of the whole project....................................................... 31
Figure 4.2 Limbic system ................................................................................ 33
Figure 4.3 Two routes of emotion.................................................................... 38
Figure 4.4 Limbic System................................................................................ 39
Figure 4.5 Brain Bone and Muscle Structure................................................... 39
Figure 4.6 fMRI result of anterior temporal region ......................................... 40
Figure 4.7 Electrode Placement ....................................................................... 41
Figure 4.8 Mild electric shock device taken from a lighter ............................. 43
Figure 4.9 Experiment sequence...................................................................... 44
Figure 4.10 PL-EEG wavepoint system........................................................... 49
Figure 4.11 3D scalp EEG mapping for independent component of anxiety
state related data.......................................................................................... 50
Figure 4.12 Plot of two-class dataset ............................................................... 54
Figure 4.13 Train-set plot and test-set plot ...................................................... 55
Figure 4.14 Resulting decision boundary of SVM and train-set or test-set data
plot .............................................................................................................. 55
Figure 5.1 ATES comparison between anxiety state and neutral emotion state 1
..................................................................................................................... 62
Figure 5.2 ATES comparison between unpleasant emotion state and neutral
emotion state 2 ............................................................................................ 62
Figure 5.3 Prefrontal Energy Spectrum in anxiety state and neutral state....... 65
vi
List of Figures
Figure 5.4 Prefrontal Energy Spectrum in negative emotion state and neutral
emotion state ............................................................................................... 65
Figure 5.5 Validation of experiment design .................................................... 70
Figure 5.6 Relationship between Training Accuracy Rate and C value of SVM
during optimization..................................................................................... 71
vii
List of Tables
LIST OF TABLES
Table 4.1 Function of components of limbic system....................................... 34
Table 4.2 The conductivity (S/m) of tissues below 100 Hz at body temperature
..................................................................................................................... 40
Table 5.1 SVM prediction result with optimal C............................................. 72
viii
1. Introduction
1. INTRODUCTION
1.1. Background
Emotion is a common phenomenon in our daily life. One common definition of
emotion in medicine is that emotion is the “mental state, periodic or dispositional,
associated with certain physiological conditions, and brought about by thoughts and
happenings perceived as desirable or undesirable.” (O’Shaughnessy 1992) An
example of a periodic emotional state is the academic’s joy at solving a tricky
intellectual conundrum; an example of an emotional dispositional state is the
sympathy that disposes people to help others. All emotional states are characterized
by bodily effects on pulse rate, blood pressure, adrenal secretion, blushing, trembling,
crying, fainting, and so on.
Many psychologists adopt the ABC model, which defines emotions in terms of three
fundamental attributes: A. physiological arousal, B. behavioral expression (e.g. facial
expressions), and C. conscious experience, the subjective feeling of an emotion.
(Myers 2004) All three attributes are necessary for a full fledged emotional event,
though the intensity of each may vary greatly. There are three major theories to
expound the relationship among these three components, which are James-Lange
Theory (James 1890), Cannon-Bard Theory (Cannon 1927) and Schacter’s
Two-factor Theory (Schachter 1971).
1
1. Introduction
James-Lange Theory, which was proposed by William James & Carl Lange, is one of
the earliest theories about emotion. In this theory, the experience of emotion is
awareness of physiological responses to emotion-arousing stimuli. The emotiontriggering stimulus notifies the sympathetic branch of the autonomic nervous system
(cause body’s arousal), and then the signal will transfer from the sympathetic branch
to the brain’s cortex, lead to subjective awareness. (Figure 1.1)
Perception
of
stimulus
Arousal
Emotion
Figure 1.1 James-Lange Theory
However, evidence for James-Lange’s theory seemed improbable because the
evidence suggested that our physiological responses are not distinct enough to evoke
different emotions. For example, does the racing heart signal mean the fear, anger,
love or excited? Also, many physiological changes happen slowly, too slowly to
trigger sudden emotional changes. So Walter Cannon & Philip Bard proposed
Cannon-Bard Theory, which is that physiological arousal and our emotional
experience occur simultaneously. The emotion-triggering stimulus notifies both the
brain’s cortex (subjective awareness) and the sympathetic branch of the autonomic
nervous system (causes body’s arousal). (Figure 1.2)
Arousal
Perception
of
stimulus
Emotion
Figure 1.2
Cannon-Bard Theory
2
1. Introduction
However, Cannon-Bard Theory didn’t explain the relationship between the emotion
and thoughts. Most psychologists today believe that our cognitions, such as our
perceptions, memories, and interpretations, are essential ingredient of emotions.
Stanley Schachter proposed his famous two-factor theory in which emotions have two
ingredients: interaction between physical arousal and cognition (“label”), which
means to experience emotion one must be both physically aroused and cognitively
label the arousal. And the physical arousal can intensify most emotions. (Figure 1.3)
Arousal
Perception
of
stimulus
Emotion
Cognitive
label
Figure 1.3
Schachter’s two-factor Theory
1.2. Problem Statements
Emotional intelligence consists of the ability to recognize, express, and have
emotions, coupled with the ability to regulate these emotions, harness them for
constructive purposes, and skillfully handle the emotions of others. The skills of
emotional intelligence have been argued to be a better predictor than IQ for
measuring aspects of success in life (D.Goleman 1995). Scientists have amassed
evidence that emotional skills are a basic component of intelligence, especially for
learning preferences and adapting to what is important (Mayer 1990; Salovey 1990;
3
1. Introduction
J.LeDoux 1996).
With increasing deployment of adaptive computer systems, the ability to sense and
respond appropriately to user emotion feedback is of growing importance. A failure to
include the emotional component in human-computer interaction is comparable to
trimming the potential bandwidth of the communication channel. Frustrating
interaction with a computer system can often leave a user feeling negatively disposed
to the system and its makers. Since humans are predisposed to respond socially to
computers, such negative experiences could alter perceptions of trust, cooperation and
good faith on the part of the user. On the other hand, enabling computers to recognize
and adapt to the user's emotion state can, in theory, improve the quality of interaction
(Preece 1994; Klein 2002; Bickmore 2004; Mishra 2004).
Due to the infinite extension of emotional phenomena, it is impossible to make a full
description of all the emotions that we can experience. So emotion is divided into two
groups: positive emotions (such as: I feel well, happy, healthy, strong, and so on) &
negative emotions (such as: I feel uncomfortable, unfortunate, sick, sad, lonely,
anxiety, and so on).
It is fair to say that not all computers need to be aware of the user's emotions because
most machines are only rigid tools. However, there is a range of areas in HCI where
computers need to adapt to their users’ emotions (Bloom 1984). Literatures on
emotion theory points out:
4
1. Introduction
Firstly, positive emotion is much harder to elicit in the laboratory in compared with
negative emotion. This phenomenon refers to the general tendency of organisms to
react more strongly to negative compared with positive stimuli, perhaps as a
consequence of evolutionary pressures to avoid harm.
Secondly, with increased levels of adrenaline and other neuron-chemicals coursing
through the body, a person engulfed by negative emotions has diminished abilities
with respect to attention, memory retention, learning, creative thinking and polite
social interaction. For example: Stress, anxiety and frustration experienced by a
learner in the educational context can degrade learning outcomes (Kahneman 1973;
Isen 1987; Lewis 1989).
Furthermore, for the safety, security and many other reasons of some careers, such as
the pilots, it is important to monitor or detect the operators’ emotion states. If the
pilots are in the state of negative emotions for a long period of time, it is more likely
for him or her to make the mistakes, which will cause tremendous loss. Thus, it is
important and useful to detect negative emotions.
1.3. Research Objectives
The main objective of this research is to propose and develop a new physical quantity,
which is named ICA-based EEG Energy Spectrum, for the features in identifying
subtle changes in the EEG signal in relationship to negative emotions. Under this
5
1. Introduction
primary objective, the detailed sub-objectives are the following:
1) To establish the analysis of this physical quantity;
2) To establish the experiments for verifying the effectiveness of this physical
quantity, which includes the protocol design, experimental design and the
critical electrodes placement design for the negative emotion detection by
using EEG;
3) To analyze the experiment results for the effectiveness of this physical
quantity;
4) To verify the results for this physical quantity by using Support Vector
Machine (SVM).
6
2. Literature Review
2. LITERATURE REVIEW
2.1 Traditional Technologies in emotion detection
Traditional technologies in emotion detection and prediction mainly focus on the
facial expression recognition, verbal signal and other physiological signals detection,
such as heart rate, respiration rate, and so on. The different emotion detection
technologies will be summarized and the specific technologies will be discussed.
2.1.1
Facial Analysis technologies
People’s facial expressions are thought to be very reliable signs of their emotional
reaction to various stimuli. The principle of this method is that different emotion has
different combination of the contractions of facial muscles. So a camera is used to
monitor several dots in the user’s face (Figure 2.1a), and each dot position represent
one special muscle contraction state (Figure 2.1b). (Ekman 1972) When the user
expresses different emotion, the relation dot position will be changed, and according
to these relation position changes, the computer will analyze and determine what
emotion state the user is now in. The well known Facial Action Coding System
(FACS) was developed by Paul Ekman and W.V. Friesen in the 1970s (Ekman 1972).
7
2. Literature Review
(a) Facial action coding point
Figure 2.1
(b) Facial muscles
Facial emotion analyses
However, several important problems(Enns 1991; He 1992; Wang 1994; Suzuki 1995;
Smilek 2000), such as the face is not in the focus of the attention, the face orientation
changing, face surface changing and the global representation of a face, can affect the
emotion detection results by this method.
2.1.2
Speech Recognition technologies
A lot of researchers work on extracting emotional content from human voice as
another technique for affective input. Speech recognition is a difficult problem in
itself. There are problems with surrounding and disturbing sounds, problems with
dialects and personality in the human voice. And if all that is solved there are also
problems with understanding the actual meaning of what is being said. The same
word can mean so many different things depending on its context and how it is being
said. Researchers have come so far that they can work with a defined set of words in a
relatively quiet environment. The emotional value of what is said and how it is said is
yet another problem to researchers. There are not yet any fully developed prototypes
8
2. Literature Review
using this method for affective input. Before that happens, researchers will have to
work on the problem of defining the characteristics of emotional states expressed in
speech. Cowie and colleagues point out the importance of working with naturally
expressed emotions and not acted data which is the most common approach (Fotinea
2003). They have noted several characteristics not previously defined such as
impaired communication and articulation. Acted data is most often based on
monologue whereas spoken emotional reactions are more common when interacting
with another part. Breakdowns and disarticulation are two examples that may not
occur in acted data. Also the patterns in pitch, volume and timing are also other
problems in the emotion detection via speech recognition.
2.1.3
Tradition methods disadvantages
There are some other methodologies based on other physiological signals to detect
emotion, such as heart beat, respiration rate, and so on. All these methods are
immature and have many problems such as low accuracy and low efficiency, and so
on. From biological basis, these physiological are all controlled by the human brain.
So EEG, which is noninvasive to directly monitor the brain signal, becomes one of
prominent alternatives to detect emotions.
2.2
EEG-Based Emotion Measurement
A large number of researches have been conducted on the emotion measurement.
9
2. Literature Review
Since Dr. Hans Berger, a German neuron-psychiatrist, published his first EEG
recording in 1929 (Berger 1929), EEG has been acclaimed as one of the most
promising tools, sensed via an array of small electrodes affixed to the scalp, and
examining alpha, beta and theta brain waves to investigate the brain function.
Particularly, with the development of computer technology, EEG plays a significant
role nowadays in the EEG-based clinical diagnosis and studies of brain function (Van
1950; Jongh 2001; Lehnertz 2001; Benar 2003; Thakor 2004). In addition, there are
various research findings showing that different mental activities, either normal or
pathological, produce different patterns of EEG signals (Miles 1996). EEG was used
to detect emotion since 1970s. And from the experiment design aspect, there are
mainly two type approaches to use EEG to detect emotion: Event-Related Potentials
(ERPs) and Cerebral Electricity Asymmetry.
2.2.1
Event-Related Potentials (ERPs)
The hypothesis of this method is that event-related potentials vary with the judged
emotionality of picture stimuli. Specifically, a widely distributed, late positive
potential (LPP) is enhanced for stimuli evaluated as distant from an established
affective context.
To test this hypothesis, Cacioppo and colleagues (Cacioppo 1993; Cacioppo 1994;
Cacioppo 1994; Cacioppo 1996) measured ERPs in response to positive and negative
pictures that were rated as equally extreme in valence and arousal. They put rare
10
2. Literature Review
emotional pictures (positive or negative) into a series of frequent neutral pictures and
showed the pictures one picture per second to the participants. At the same time, the
EEG signal was recorded from F3, Fz, F4, C3, C4, P3, Pz, P4, A1, and A2 of
international 10-20 EEG standard electrode positions (Figure 2.2). After that,
participants were instructed to evaluate the pictures and to report their evaluations
after the picture disappeared. The result was that a pleasant target stimulus presented
within a series of unpleasant pictures elicits a larger LPP than does the same pleasant
target, presented among other pleasant stimuli. (Figure 2.3)
Figure 2.2
International 10-20 EEG standard electrode positions
Similar results are found for unpleasant targets (in a pleasant series) for this affective
oddball paradigm. Furthermore, the greater the affective distance of a target (the
greater its valence difference from the series) the larger the late potential. These
findings appear to parallel results obtained with conventional, non-affective oddball
tasks, in which a rare stimulus event (e.g. a high tone preceded by a series of low
tones) elicits larger late positivity (P300) than a stimulus consistent with the context
(Donchin 1988). The LPP in the affective oddball paradigm differs somewhat from
the traditional, non-affective P300 in that it usually occurs later, and appears to be
11
2. Literature Review
partially lateralized-with larger LPP amplitudes over the right than the left parietal
hemisphere (Cacioppo 1994).
Figure 2.3
Using ERPs to differentiate negative/positive emotions
However, positive and negative pictures do not produce qualitatively different
responses, such as there is no different direction of ERPs, and there are no different
ERPs in different locations, and so on. Hence, ERPs can at best represent the arousal
dimension of emotion, but not the valence dimension. Moreover, a similar positive
activation is found for any rare stimuli in a series of frequent stimuli (e.g., a high tone
in a series of low tones). Hence, the ERPs may reflect surprise, but not emotional
responses to the content of the pictures. Thus, it is not suitable to use ERPs to detect
or measure the negative emotions.
12
2. Literature Review
2.2.2
Cerebral Electricity Asymmetry
The other main approach is based on the cerebral electricity asymmetry for emotional
processes. Since 1970s, scientists have found that there is cerebral lateralization for
emotional processes which have two main formulations. The results of some studies
(Carmon 1973; Gardner 1975; Davidson 1976; A 1977) seemed to suggest that the
right hemisphere was more involved than the left in subserving emotional processes.
Other studies (Gainotti 1972; Dimond 1977; Ahern 1979; L 1985), however, have
suggested the existence of a differential lateralization for positive and negative
emotion, in which the left hemisphere is more involved in the mediation of positive
emotion and the right hemisphere is more involved in the mediation of negative
emotion.
More and more researchers (Masaoka 2000; Davidson 2001; Hariri 2003; Davidson
2004; Hare 2005) supported the second hypothesis. Using a variety of methods to
make inferences about regionally specific patterns of activation, many investigators
have now reported systematic asymmetries in patterns of activation in specific brain
regions in response to certain types of positive and negative emotional challenges.
For example, Schmidt et al (Schmidt 2002) measured EEG asymmetries while
participants were listening to positive (happy) and negative (fear/sad) musical
excerpts. The EEG signal was collected from F3, F4, Cz, P3 and P4 and two more
electrodes were used to detect EOG. All the collected EEG data were visually scored
13
2. Literature Review
for artifact due to eye blinks, eye movements, and other motor movements and all
artifact-free EEG data were analyzed using a discrete Fourier transform (DFT), with a
Hanning window of 1s width and 50% overlap. Power (micro-volts-squared) was
derived from the DFT output in the alpha band (8-13 Hz); a natural log (ln)
transformation was performed on the EEG data to reduce skewness. As expected,
happy music increased left-right asymmetries, whereas sad and fearful music
decreased left-right activity. (Figure 2.4)
Figure 2.4 Emotion detection using brain asymmetry
As we know that alpha power is inversely related to activity, thus lower power
reflects more activity. So for negative emotions (fear and sad), the left hemisphere
frontal alpha power is larger than the right hemisphere frontal alpha power, which
means left hemisphere frontal activity is less than the right hemisphere frontal activity
in negative emotions. So in positive emotions (joy and happy) the left hemisphere
frontal activity is larger than the right hemisphere frontal activity.
14
2. Literature Review
Despite the complexities associated with aggregating studies with vastly different
experimental designs, a recent meta-analytic review has also supported the notion that
certain forms of positive and negative emotion exhibit different patterns of functional
brain asymmetry, particularly in prefrontal cortical territories.
Based on a large body of both human and animal experiment studies, Davidson and
his colleagues (Davidson 2003) have proposed that greater left-sided dorsolateral
activity may be associated with approach-related, goal-directed action planning,
whereas on a lesser level of consensus, from the neuron-imaging studies of spatial
working memory, they suggested that activation of right lateral prefrontal cortex
during withdrawal-related emotion may be associated with threat-related vigilance.
Davidson also reported that positive and negative emotion states shift the asymmetry
in prefrontal brain electrical activity in lawful ways. For example, film-induced
negative emotion i.e. fear/anxiety increases relative right-sided prefrontal cortex
activation, whereas induced positive emotion elicits an opposite pattern of
asymmetric activation (Davidson 2003).
Furthermore, Heller and colleagues have proposed that asymmetries in parietal cortex
may be associated with arousal such that greater right-sided posterior activation is
associated with higher arousal emotion. And subjects exhibit stable differences in
asymmetric patterns of activation in prefrontal brain regions that predict various
features of affective reactivity. However, there are several issues regarding the
“asymmetry” works.
15
2. Literature Review
Firstly, all previous emotion detection by using EEG is based on electrical asymmetry
by measuring alpha band power. However, as we know, there are several factors
which can affect the alpha band power, such as attention shifting, fatigue level
changing, and so on. Furthermore, Mueller (1999) has reported that right frontal sites
exhibited a significant increase in power for positive and negative valence relative to
neutral stimuli for γ-40 power compared to the neutral condition, and also no
statistically significant effect was found for alpha activity in anxiety state, indicating
no sensitivity of alpha de-synchronization. All these arguments weaken the possibility
of negative emotion detection by electrical asymmetry.
Secondly, all the researchers collected the EEG signal from the prefrontal and parietal
surface (such as Fz, Pz, and Cz). For the prefrontal, the main function of prefrontal
cortex (PFC) is the executive function, which means PFC has more complex signal
that mix the signals related to emotion with other signals non-related to emotion. For
the parietal, the primary sensory cortex and primary motor cortex lies there, which
means parietal also has more complex signal. So it is not practical to get the emotion
EEG data from prefrontal cortex or parietal part.
The third disadvantage is the signal processing methodology. Human eyes were used
to recognize and grade those obvious noises, such as eye blinking, muscle movement.
However, the traditional signal processing method can not work for the artifacts
which have the same amplitude with the emotion-related EEG data. Also, frequency
domain analysis was the main method used to analyze the results; however, there are
16
2. Literature Review
no consistent results from different researcher, which may because of this
ineffectiveness of this signal processing method.
Thus, considered the complexity of EEG signals, Independent Component Analysis
(ICA) has been investigated as well as the biological basis of emotion and brain
structure, a novel ICA-based EEG Energy Spectrum was proposed and used to
evaluate some negative emotions, such as anxiety.
17
3. ICA-based EEG Energy Spectrum
3. ICA-based EEG Energy Spectrum
This chapter describes the biological basis of ICA-based EEG Energy Spectrum, as
well as the principle of ICA-based EEG Energy Spectrum, which includes
Independent Component Analysis, Scalp EEG mapping, and ICA-based EEG Energy
Spectrum calculation.
3.1
Biological Basis
As we know, different kinds of brain activities are the result of some neuron groups
firing in the certain time sequence and certain intensity. The neuron groups’ firing
implies the neurons activation, which will cause peak electrical potentials to appear at
specific locations on the scalp. Figure 3.1 shows some brain activities with different
neurons firing pattern.
Figure 3.1
Some Brain Activities
18
3. ICA-based EEG Energy Spectrum
For simplification, each of this activated neuron group can be viewed as one electrical
source and all the electrical sources are independent on each other. Thus, by
summarizing the peak electrical potentials appearing in the specific locations on the
scalp in the certain time slot, the intensity of neuron groups’ activation related to
some brain activity can be determined. Here the intensity of neuron groups’ firing
represents the neurons activation energy.
Therefore, the specific brain activity can be monitored or measured by the number of
the “peak” electrical potentials appearing in the specific locations on the scalp, and
this forms the basis of ICA-based EEG Energy Spectrum. Under this principle, the
calculation of ICA-based EEG Energy Spectrum consists of four steps: Independent
Component Analysis, Scalp EEG Mapping, Brain Activity Classification and
Statistical Analysis.
3.2
Independent Component Analysis (ICA)
Independent component analysis (ICA) is a computational method for separating a
multivariate signal into additive subcomponents supposing the mutual statistical
independence of the non-Gaussian source signals. This method is mainly for the blind
source separation (Herault 1991; Common 1994), in which case the original
independent sources are assumed to be unknown, and yet to be separated from their
19
3. ICA-based EEG Energy Spectrum
weighted mixtures. Furthermore, modeling of noise or artifacts is not required in ICA.
Figure 3.2 is the illustration of Independent Component Analysis.
Figure 3.2
3.2.1
Illustration of Independent Component Analysis
ICA Algorithm
The basic data model used in defining (linear) ICA assumes that the observed
n-dimensional data vector at time instant t, x(t) = [x1(t),…, xn(t)]T is given by
m
X(t ) = ∑ ai si (t ) = As(t )
(3.1)
i =1
where s(t) = [s1(t), … , sm(t)]T are m independent source signals with zero mean,
which can be guaranteed by explicitly extracting the mean of each xi(t) without loss
of generality, and A = [a1, … , am] is a constant mixing matrix which is a function of
the location of the sources, the positioning in an EEG recording, the shape and the
conductivity distribution of the brain as a volume conductor (Vigario 1997). As in the
general blind signal separation problem, A is assumed to be an n×m matrix of full
rank (there are at least as many mixtures as the number of independent sources, i.e. n
20
3. ICA-based EEG Energy Spectrum
> m). In addition, although A is unknown, we assume it to be constant, or
semi-constant (preserving local constancy) in order to perform ICA.
If W denotes the inverse or pseudo-inverse of A, the problem can be redefined
equivalently as to find the separating matrix W that satisfies
s(t ) = Wx(t )
(3.2)
It has been documented that the preprocessing the input data (mixtures) by whitening
can significantly ease the separation of the source signals (Karhunen 1997). Therefore,
in the first step, we implement standard principal component analysis (PCA) for
whitening x. It can be shown in the compact form (noting that we have now dropped
the time index t):
v = Vx
(3.3)
where E{vvT} = I with I denotes the unit matrix. The whitening matrix V is given by
V = D−1/ 2 ET
(3.4)
where D = diag[λ1, … , λm] is a diagonal matrix with the eigenvalues of covariance
matrix E{xixiT} as its diagonal elements, and E is a matrix with the corresponding
eigenvectors as its columns.
The key to estimating the independent components from their mixtures by using ICA
is non-Gaussianity. Intuitively speaking, maximizing the norm of this kurtosis leads
to the separation of one non-Gaussian source from the observed mixtures. In our
algorithm, non-Gaussianity is measured by the classical fourth-order cumulant or
kurtosis. Consider y = wTv, with ||w|| = 1, kurtosis is calculated by
21
3. ICA-based EEG Energy Spectrum
kurt (y ) = E{(y ) 4 } − 3[ E{(y ) 2 }]2
(3.5)
where operator E denotes the mathematical expectation.
Then the FastICA fixed-point algorithm based on gradient descent searching
(Hyvarinen 1999; Hyvarinen 2000) is used to search the expectation maximization.
As a result, rows of the separating matrix W and corresponding independent sources
are identified one by one, up to a maximum of m. The basic steps of this efficient
algorithm are as follows:
1) Choose initial vector w0 randomly (iteration step l=0).
2) Let wl = E{v(wl-1Tv)3}-3wl-1.
3) Let wl=wl/||wl||.
If the stop criterion has not been satisfied, the program will go back to step 2. Due to
the cubic convergence of the algorithm, the solution is typically found in less than 15
iterations. Figure 3.3 shows an example of independent component analysis.
500
T6
0
-500
0
500
O2
500
600
700
800
900
1000
100
200
300
400
500
600
700
800
900
1000
100
200
300
400
500
600
700
800
900
1000
100
200
300
400
500
600
700
800
900
1000
100
200
300
400
500
600
700
800
900
1000
100
200
300
400
500
600
700
800
900
1000
0
-500
0
500
P3
400
0
-500
0
500
O1
300
0
-1000
0
500
Pz
200
0
-500
0
1000
P4
100
0
-500
0
(a) twelve seconds of anxiety emotion state EEG raw data
22
3. ICA-based EEG Energy Spectrum
10
C1
0
-10
0
5
C2
500
600
700
800
900
1000
100
200
300
400
500
600
700
800
900
1000
100
200
300
400
500
600
700
800
900
1000
100
200
300
400
500
600
700
800
900
1000
100
200
300
400
500
600
700
800
900
1000
100
200
300
400
500
600
700
800
900
1000
0
-5
0
5
C6
400
0
-5
0
5
C5
300
0
-2
0
5
C4
200
0
-5
0
2
C3
100
0
-5
0
(b) ICs of the 12 seconds of anxiety emotion state EEG raw data
Figure 3.3
3.3
Independent Component Analysis
Scalp EEG Mapping
After independent component analysis, the artifacts and noises can be easily
identified, such as in Figure 3.3 (b), the heartbeat (C1) and the environment noise (C3)
can be removed directly. For other components, such as C4, C5 and C6, it is very
difficult to identify the brain activity from them. In order to classify all these
components, Scalp EEG mapping is introduced to visualize all the components. There
are four steps in the EEG mapping: grid generation, interpolation, equivalence
contour calculation and color bar scaling.
23
3. ICA-based EEG Energy Spectrum
3.3.1
Grid generation
In order to represent the power distribution on a coordinator system independent of
the electrode position systems, a grid of spherical coordinator system (Figure 3.4) is
used. Select proper m and n, all electrodes of international 10-20 system will coincide
with grid points; it will help to improve the accuracy of interpolation. And the power
distribution is represented by the power values at the grid points. The power value at
each grid is determined from the power values of neighboring electrodes by
interpolation.
(a) Spherical Coordinator system
(b) Generated grid
Figure 3.4
Grid generation
24
3. ICA-based EEG Energy Spectrum
3.3.2
Interpolation
Generally, linear interpolation is adopted to calculate the grid value. Each grid value
is determined by the neighboring electrodes. Figure 3.5 shows the example of linear
interpolation.
Figure 3.5 Illustration of linear interpolation
3.3.3
Equivalent contour calculation
After interpolation, the value of every grid in the spherical coordinator system has
been calculated and compared. Thus, equivalent contour can be drawn. (Figure 3.6)
Figure 3.6
Illustration of equivalent contour
25
3. ICA-based EEG Energy Spectrum
3.3.4
Color bar scaling
Self-scale method has been adopted to determine the color value of the equivalent
contour. In this method, every independent component’s coefficient values in all the
electrode position are compared and the color value is determined according to the
scaling algorism. (Figure 3.7) After color scaling, the equivalent contour become
colored. Figure 3.8 is one example of scalp EEG map, which indicates a special
activation pattern in left anterior temporal region.
Figure 3.7
Figure 3.8
Color scaling algorism
Example of Scalp EEG map
26
3. ICA-based EEG Energy Spectrum
3.4
ICA-based EEG Energy Spectrum
For Scalp EEG maps of ICA result, it can be both 2D scalp EEG map and 3D scalp
EEG map. (Figure 3.9) So after ICA and scalp EEG mapping, all the independent
components can be compared with each other according to their activation pattern.
Figure 3.9 Scalp EEG mapping for the ICA results
Furthermore, in order to investigate the brain activation pattern in detailed, 3D scalp
EEG mapping for one independent component was computed in four directions:
top-frontal, top-behind, top-left and top-right. (Figure 3.10)
27
3. ICA-based EEG Energy Spectrum
Figure 3.10
Four direction view of 3D scalp EEG mapping for the ICA result
Figure 3.11 classification of 3D scalp EEG mapping for the ICA results
28
3. ICA-based EEG Energy Spectrum
After EEG scalp EEG mapping for all the independent components, we can define
several activation pattern labels, such as Left Prefrontal Cortex Activation, Right
Prefrontal Cortex Activation, and so on. And all the independent components will be
classified into these labels. (Figure 3.11 shows the example of classification of 3D
scalp EEG mapping for the ICA results)
After classification, the peak activation points in each independent component in
specific scalp region can be summarized according to the classification. This
summarized point is the Energy Spectrum. Let’s take the example of Left Prefrontal
Energy Spectrum. From the brain structure and international 10-20 system, the left
prefrontal region covers Fp1 and F7. So the definition of Left Prefrontal Energy
Spectrum is the following:
Left Prefrontal Energy Spectrum (LPES) is the total number of activation points in all
independent components which have peak activation in Fp1 or F7. In order words,
independent components with peak activations at Fp1 or F7 would be considered to
have left prefrontal cortex activation. In such cases, for every independent component
which has activation in Fp1 or F7, the data tally for Left Prefrontal Energy Spectrum
for that participant would be increased by one if only Fp1 or F7 has the peak
activation and would be increased by two if both Fp1 and F7 have the peak activation.
For the example showed in Figure 3.10, the LPES is 1 because only F7 is the peak
activation.
29
3. ICA-based EEG Energy Spectrum
To sum up, Scalp EEG relates to the energy of neuronal activation in the brain. ICA
gives independent components which are associated with specific neuronal activation
sources. From the scalp EEG mapping of each independent component, the peak
electrical activation in the specific area indicates that the neurons in that region are
activated. Thus, the summarized peak electrical points in a specific scalp region from
all the independent components will indicate the energy of the neuron group’s
activation nearby that region.
30
4. Experimental Design
4
EXPERIMENTAL DESIGN
The overall objective of this research is to propose and develop a new physical
quantity for the features in identifying subtle changes in the EEG signal in
relationship to negative emotions. In last chapter, the basis and principle of this
quantity has been discussed. Thus, this chapter will describe the experimental design
for negative emotion detection by using this quantity. Figure 4.1 shows the overall
flowchart of the experiment.
Protocol
Design
EEG Signal
Acquisition
Figure 4.1
Signal
Processing
Feature
Identification
Flowchart of the whole project
In this chapter, the biological basis of emotion for the experiments, experiment
protocol design, the experiment materials, signal processing method and results
verification method used for this research will be discussed.
4.1 Biological Basis of Emotion
From biological aspect, the structures in the human brain involved in emotion,
motivation, and emotional association with memory belong to the limbic system,
which influences the formation of memory by integrating emotional states with stored
memories of physical sensations.
31
4. Experimental Design
The French physician Paul Broca first called this part of the brain "le grand lobe
limbique" in 1878, but its putative role in emotion was not largely developed until
1937, when the American physician James Papez first described his anatomical model
of emotion, which is still referred to as the Papez circuit. Papez's ideas were, in turn,
later expanded on by Paul D. MacLean to include additional structures in a more
dispersed "limbic system," more on the lines of the system described above. The
concept of the limbic system has since been further expanded and developed by
Nauta, Heimer and others.
4.1.1
Emotion Loop
In 1937, the neuroanatomist James Papez (Papez 1937) would demonstrate that
emotion is not a function of any specific brain center but of a circuit that involves
four basic structures, interconnected through several nervous bundles: the
hypothalamus with its mamillary bodies, the anterior thalamic nucleus, the cingulate
gyrus and the hippocampus. Papez believed that the experience of emotion was
primarily determined by the cingulate cortex and, secondly, by other cortical areas.
Emotional expression was thought to be governed by the hypothalamus. The
cingulate gyrus projects to the hippocampus and the hippocampus projects to the
hypothalamus by way of the bundle of axons called fornix. Hypothalamic impulses
reach the cortex via relay in the anterior thalamic nuclei. This circuit (Papez circuit),
acting in a harmonic fashion, is responsible for the central functions of emotion
(affect), as well as for its peripheral expressions (symptoms). In 1949, Paul McLean
32
4. Experimental Design
completed and corrected Papez’s ideas, and called the larger complex the limbic
system, which is what we call it today (Maclean 1952). It included the hypothalamus,
the hippocampus, and the amygdala, and is tightly connected with the cingulate gyrus,
the ventral tegmental area of the brain stem, the septum, and the prefrontal gyrus.
Figure 4.2 shows the basic structure of limbic system.
Figure 4.2
4.1.2
Limbic system
Function of limbic system
By influencing the endocrine system and the autonomic nervous system, the limbic
system is highly interconnected with a structure known as the nucleus accumbens,
commonly called the brain's pleasure center. The nucleus accumbens plays a role in
sexual arousal and the "high" derived from certain recreational drugs. These
responses are heavily modulated by dopaminergic projections from the limbic system.
In 1954, Olds and Milner found that rats with metal electrodes implanted into their
nucleus accumbens would repeatedly press a lever activating this region, and would
do so in preference to eating and drinking, eventually dying of exhaustion (Olds
1954).
33
4. Experimental Design
The limbic system is also tightly connected to the prefrontal cortex. Some scientists
contend that this connection is related to the pleasure obtained from solving problems.
To cure severe emotional disorders, this connection was sometimes surgically severed,
a procedure of psychosurgery, called a prefrontal lobotomy (this is actually a
misnomer). Patients who underwent this procedure often became passive and lacked
all motivations.
There is circumstantial evidence that the limbic system also provides a custodial
function for the maintenance of a healthy conscious state of mind. For each
component in limbic system, the detailed functions are listed in Table 4.1. (Lautin
2001)
Table 4.1
Function of components of limbic system
Structure
Amygdala
Cingulate gyrus
Fornicate gyrus
Hippocampus
Hypothalamus
Mammillary body
Nucleus accumbens
Orbitofrontal cortex
Parahippocampal gyrus
Function
Involved in aggression, jealousy, and fear
Autonomic functions regulating heart rate and blood
pressure as well as cognitive and attention processing
Region encompassing the cingulate , hippocampus , and
parahippocampal gyrus
Required for the formation of long-term memories
Regulates the autonomic nervous system via hormone
production and release. Affects and regulates blood
pressure, heart rate, hunger, thirst, sexual arousal, and the
sleep/wake cycle
Important for the formation of memory
Involved in reward, pleasure, and addiction
Required for decision making
Plays a role in the formation of spatial memory
34
4. Experimental Design
4.1.3
Key components of limbic system
With recent advances in functional brain imaging (fMRI and PET), the circuitry
underlying emotion in the human brain can now be studied with unprecedented
precision. Two basic systems (approach system and withdrawal system) mediating
different forms of motivation and emotion has been proposed (Lang 1990; Gray 1994;
Davidson 1995). Although the descriptors chosen by different investigators vary and
the specifics of the proposed anatomical circuitry are presented in varying levels of
detail,
the
essential
characteristics
of
each
system
are
similar
across
conceptualizations. The approach system facilitates appetitive behavior and generates
certain types of positive affect that are approach-related, for example, enthusiasm,
pride, etc. This form of positive emotion is usually generated in the context of moving
toward a desired goal. There appears to be a second system concerned with the neural
implementation of withdrawal. This system facilitates the withdrawal of an individual
from sources of aversive stimulation and generates certain forms of negative emotion
that are withdrawal-related. For example, both fear and disgust are associated with
increasing the distance between the organism and a source of aversive stimulation. A
variety of evidence drawn from multiple sources suggests the view that the systems
that support these forms of positive and negative emotion are implemented in
partially separable neural circuits. Recent studies have shown that amygdale and
prefrontal cortex (PFC) are key structures in the circuit that govern positive and
negative affect (Davidson 1992; Coleman-Mesches K 1995; Zald DH 1997; Zald DH
1998).
35
4. Experimental Design
A large corpus of data at both the animal and human levels implicates various sectors
of the PFC in emotion. The PFC is the anterior part of the frontal lobes of the brain,
and is lying in front of the motor and premotor areas. Cytoarchitectonically, it is
defined by the presence of an internal granular layer IV (in contrast to the agranular
premotor cortex). This brain region has been implicated in Executive Function, which
includes planning complex cognitive behaviors, personality expression, moderating
correct social behavior, and the abilities to differentiate between conflicting thoughts,
determine good and bad, better and best, same and different, future consequences of
current activities, working toward a defined goal, prediction of outcomes, expectation
based on actions, and so on.
The amygdale is a brain structure that is essential for decoding emotions, and in
particular stimuli that are threatening to the organism. When the brain receives a
sensory stimulus indicating a danger, it is routed first to the sensory thalamus. From
there, the information is sent out over two parallel pathways: the thalamo-amygdala
pathway (the “short route”), which is fast, but involuntary and imprecise route, and
the thalamo-cortico-amygdala pathway (the “long route”), which is slow, but
voluntary and precise route.(Kandel E. R. 2000; Maren 2001)
The short route conveys a fast, rough impression of the situation, because it is a
sub-cortical pathway in which no cognition is involved. This pathway activates the
amygdala which, through its central nucleus, generates emotional responses before
any perceptual integration has even occurred and before the mind can form a
36
4. Experimental Design
complete representation of the stimulus. This route is important because it lets us start
preparing for a potential danger before we even know exactly what it is. In some
situations, these precious fractions of a second can mean the difference between life
and death.
The long route conveys the information from the sensory thalamus to the cortex. First,
the various modalities of the perceived object are processed by the primary sensory
cortex. Then the unimodal associative cortex provides the amygdala with a
representation of the object. At an even higher level of analysis, the polymodal
associative cortex conceptualizes the object and also informs the amygdala about it.
This elaborate representation of the object is then compared with the contents of
explicit memory by means of the hippocampus, which also communicates closely
with the amygdala. After processing, the information will reach amygdale again and
tell the amygdale whether or not the stimulus represents a real threat. (Figure 4.3)
The imminent presence of a danger then performs the task of activating the amygdala,
whose discharge patterns in turn activate the efferent structures responsible for
physical manifestations of fear, such as increased heart rate and blood pressure,
sweaty hands, dry mouth, and tense muscles.
Based on the above discussion, there are several locations for emotion measurement
by EEG, where to collect the emotion related EEG signal will be discussed in the
following section.
37
4. Experimental Design
Figure 4.3
Two routes of emotion
4.2 EEG Electrode Placements
In this research, the EEG electrode placement is based on international 10-20 system.
However, the brain structure which is involved in the limbic system (Figure 4.4) has
two suitable locations, one of which is the temporal pole which belongs to paralimbic
system and also connects to Amygdala and hippocampus group. The other location is
the prefrontal lobe.
Based on the brain bone and muscle structure (Figure 4.5) and the conductivity (S/m)
of body tissues below 100 Hz at body temperature (Table 4.2), the prefrontal lobe can
not be a suitable location to detect emotion by using EEG because of two reasons.
38
4. Experimental Design
One is that the prefrontal bone is thicker than other head bones and also the bone has
the lowest conductance. The other reason is that the prefrontal region has more
complex higher function, thus it is difficulty to differentiate emotion related EEG
signal from other type of EEG signal.
Figure 4.4 Limbic System
Figure 4.5 Brain Bone and Muscle Structure
So the temporal pole will be considered in this study. Moreover there are some fMRI
evidences to support that when the subjects are in the negative emotion states, such as
39
4. Experimental Design
anxiety, there are neurons activation in the anterior temporal region or temporal pole.
(Figure 4.6) So four electrodes, X1, X2, X3 and X4, are adopted to collect EEG
signal from the temporal pole (Figure 4.7)
Table 4.2
The conductivity (S/m) of tissues below 100 Hz at body temperature
Tissue
Bone -Marrow
Cartilage
Fat
Muscle
Blood
Figure 4.6
Human body
0.05
0.18
0.04
0.35
0.7
Tissue
Cerebellum
Colon
White Matter
Grey Matter
Bone -Cortical
Human body
0.1
0.1
0.06
0.1
0.02
fMRI result of anterior temporal region
40
4. Experimental Design
Figure 4.7 Electrode Placement
(a) Right side view of the electrode placement. (b) Top view of the electrode
placement. X1, X2, X3 and X4 are four additional electrodes which are the sites on
the scalp close to the anterior temporal region and are not covered by the international
10-20 electrode placement system. X1 and X3 are on the left hemisphere and X2 and
X4 are on the right side of the head. X2 is attached to the elongating line of T6 and
T4, and the distance between T6 and T4 is the same as the distance between T4 and
X2. X4 is attached just above the zygomatic process, posterior to the temples by
approximately 2/3 of the total length from the temple to the ear.
4.3 Experimental Protocol
How to induce the participants to produce the emotion naturally in the laboratory
environment is a key factor for emotion detection. There are several different
methods to induce emotions in the laboratory setting. Some research group uses free
recall e.g. to ask the participant to relive a situation where they felt anger (Frijda 1989;
Mauro 1992). Other stimulus includes videos or computer games like X-quest (van
Reekum 2004) and GAME (Kaiser 1996). In this research, several different types of
stimulus, such as International Affective Picture System (IAPS), electrical shocks,
have been used to induce the participants to produce different type emotions.
41
4. Experimental Design
4.3.1
International Affective Picture System (IAPS)
IAPS is one of the most widely used emotion stimulus, which consists of over 940
standardized static pictures (Lang 1988; Lang 2005). They are classified with two
main rating categories – Valence and Arousal. Valence rating measures the degree of
pleasantness and arousal rating measures the intensity of activation. Valence and
activation are two separate and orthogonal characteristics of emotion. These ratings
are highly correlated between the participants and are verified several times. The
rating ranges from 1 (low) – 9 (high). For example, picture of a baby or a couple
hugging is of high valence i.e. pleasant pictures; picture of mushroom or stool is of
neutral valence i.e. neutral pictures and low valence or unpleasant pictures are
pictures of violence or burnt victims.
Large literature has shown that IAPS is reliable in inducing emotions. (Müller 1999)
IAPS are also used in research for self reported emotion (Davis 1995), effects on
corrugator muscle activity, skin conductance responses and heart rate (Bradley 2001)
as well as effects of the IAPS on the rating of affective words (Lang 1998)
In this experiment, the slideshows will present each IAPS picture for 6 seconds with
the exception of the emotion control stage with a 5 seconds starting slide. This is a
general common procedure for the use of IAPS in research. The slideshow didn’t
have any indication of the emotion they will induce, this is to avoid demand
characteristics and minimize anticipation of the pictures. Also, erotic images were left
42
4. Experimental Design
out of the slides as there could be complications of different emotion induced among
males and females (Bradley 2001).
4.3.2
Electrical shocks
Another stimulus used in this experiment was the electric shock device. The
mechanism in the lighter which produce a small spark was used to produce a harmless
stimulus to the participants (Figure 4.8). It is imperative that the shock device can
produce a sharp and painful shock as it acts as a punishment for them to induce
anxiety.
Figure 4.8
4.3.3
Mild electric shock device taken from a lighter
Overview Protocol
The whole experiment involves 4 main stages I)
Positive/pleasant Emotions (PE)
II)
Neutral Emotions (NE)
43
4. Experimental Design
III)
Negative/unpleasant emotion (UE)
IV)
Anxiety (AX)
The sequence of the experiment is designed to stimulate positive emotions before
negative emotions as it is believed that physiological activities due to negative
emotions persist longer than positive emotions (Thayerb 2003). The experiment
sequence and the approximated time taken for each stage are summarized in Figure
4.9.
START
2 minutes of BL
5 minutes of UE
5 minutes break
2 minutes of BL
5 minutes of PE
5 minutes break
5 tra ils o f
2 m in u tes of N E +
2 m in u tes of A X
Figure 4.9
5 minutes break
2 minutes of BL
5 minutes of NE
END
Experiment sequence
One experiment lasts approximately 2 hours including the setup time of the EEG
system. Each stage is conducted one after another with 5 minutes break in between.
This break is necessary for the participant to rest and recover from the stimulus in the
previous stage. The experiment is recorded using a video camera to help to
synchronize the extraction of the EEG data. Segments of raw EEG data are extracted
by checking the facial expression and the body language of the participant in the
video.
44
4. Experimental Design
For the first 3 stages, the stimuli used are series of IAPS pictures. These pictures are
shown, with the lights switched off, using a 17 inch color monitor placed
approximately 0.5m away from the participant. The participant is isolated to one
corner of the room with the experimenter standing behind to minimize any form of
contact. This will help the participant to concentrate on performing the experiment.
He/she is reminded to pay attention to the slideshows and keep their eyes open during
each of stage. At the start of each stage, 2 minutes of baseline (BL) data is taken for
comparison. The first two stages, PE and NE, are the two controls for the experiment.
The pictures chosen will induce positive emotions and neutral emotions respectively.
The third stage, UE, subjects the participant to unpleasant emotions. In the final stage,
the participants are required to mentally calculate mathematical for 2 minutes when
Neutral Emotion State EEG data will be colleted. After that, the participant will be
told that the electrical shocks will be randomly delivered on the left or right hand
sometimes. At the same time, the Anxiety related EEG signal will be collected.
4.3.4
Detailed Protocol
Stage I – Positive Emotion (PE)
In this stage, 2 minutes of baseline (BL) are first recorded. The participant will be
asked to look at the blank screen. After which, the slideshow will be played and
participant will be asked to view the slideshow.
45
4. Experimental Design
The slideshow consists of pictures are selected from IAPS. They are of high valence
rating (Mval
≅ 7.5) and moderate-high arousal rating (Maro ≅ 5.3). The slideshow is 5
minutes long and has 50 pictures. Each picture is shown continuously for 6 seconds.
After the slideshow ended, participant will be told to rest for 5minutes
Stage II –Neutral Emotion (NE)
Similar to Stage I, 2 minutes of baseline (BL) are first recorded. The participant will
be asked to look at the blank screen. After which, the slideshow will be played and
participant will be asked to view the slideshow.
The slideshow consists of pictures are selected from IAPS. They are of moderate
valence rating (Mval
≅ 5.2) and low arousal rating (Maro ≅ 2.8). The slideshow is 5
minutes long and has 50 pictures. Each picture is shown continuously for 6 seconds.
After the slideshow ended, participant will be told to rest for 5minutes
Stage III –Unpleasant Emotion (UE)
Similar to Stage I and II, 2 minutes of baseline (BL) are first recorded. The
participant will be asked to look at the blank screen. After which, the slideshow will
be played and participant will be asked to view the slideshow.
Pictures are selected from IAPS. They are of low valence rating (M valence
≅ 1.9) and
moderate to high arousal rating (M arousal ≅ 6.2). The slideshow is 5 minutes long and
46
4. Experimental Design
has 50 pictures. Each picture is shown continuously for 6 seconds. After the
slideshow ended, participant will be told to rest for 5minutes
Stage IV – Anxiety (AX)
In this stage, the participant starts to mentally multiply numbers for two minutes.
Neutral Emotion EEG data 1 (termed as NE1) is collected. Then the participants will
be told that an electric pulse will be delivered on the left or right hand sometime over
the next 2 minutes. Electric shock from a small spark emitter is delivered after 2
minutes. Anxiety Present EEG Data 1 (termed as AX1) is collected. Inform
participant that 2 minutes are up and let participant rest for 1 minute and collect the
NE2 in another 1 minute when the subject is mentally multiplying numbers. Repeated
the above processes, and AX2, NE3, AX3, NE4, AX4, NE5, AX5 are collected.
4.4
Experimental Materials
4.4.1
Experiment Participants
Eight right-handed healthy young adults (age range 19-23) were recruited from the
National University of Singapore for the experiment. Prior to this, five pilot
experiments with different participants have been used to verify the experimental
procedures. Using the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield 1971), they are
checked to ensure that they are right hand dominant. Experimental exclusion of left
47
4. Experimental Design
hand dominant participants is due to the different hemispheric specialization of the
brain Though it is easier to induce emotions, more particularly negative emotions, in
females participants, the experiment will extend this research to males as well. Hence,
among the participants, four subjects are males and the rest are females. To qualify
for the study, subjects had to have no medical contraindications such as severe
concomitant disease, alcoholism, drug abuse, and psychological or intellectual
problems likely to limit compliance. Before the experiment, the participants are
briefed of the general protocol and they are asked to sign the informed consent.
Throughout the session, they are constantly reminded to minimize body movement
and remain silent to reduce any noise in the EEG data. Each participant will perform
the whole experiment in a single session. This is to minimize any variables, such as
the impedance values of the electrodes, if each stage is conducted in separate sessions.
After the whole experiment, they will be asked to fill in a subjective rating form.
4.4.2
EEG Machine
The commercial EEG machines “PL-EEG Wavepoint system” (Medtronic, Inc.
Denmark) (Figure 4.10) with reusable cup electrodes was used to conduct these
experiments. Electrodes were placed using ELEFIX EEG paste and SKINPURE skin
preparation gel, both products of NIHON KOHDEN. The EEG machine has a
frequency band of 0.1-30Hz, 167 sampling frequency and 30 channel input. During
all EEG testing, electrical impedances at all electrode sites were less than 13 KΩ.
48
4. Experimental Design
Figure 4.10
4.5
PL-EEG wavepoint system
Signal Processing Methods
For each participant, 5 sets of Negative emotion state EEG data (anxiety emotion and
unpleasant emotion) and 5 sets of Control Emotion state EEG data (Neutral Emotion)
have been collected for the analyzing. The participants are observed carefully for
signs of significant distress or hints of anxiety, upon which the time is noted down.
In each of these five sets, twelve seconds of mixed EEG data at which the participant
seems to experience the most anxiety or unpleasant, is extracted. It was determined
arbitrarily to use twelve seconds for each sampled data because twelve seconds is
deemed enough time for a distinct anxiety characteristic to be accentuated, yet not too
long a period such that other artifacts becomes apparent.
The fast fixed-point algorithm for independent component analysis (FastICA) in
Matlab was invoked to conduct Independent Component Analysis. FastICA is an
49
4. Experimental Design
efficient and popular algorithm for independent component analysis invented by
Aapo Hyvärinen at Helsinki University of Technology. The algorithm is based on a
fixed-point iteration scheme maximizing non-gaussianity as a measure of statistical
independence. It can be also derived as an approximate Newton iteration. FastICA
separates the mixed signals into distinct, characteristic components independent of
one another.
EEGLab was invoked to create 3D maps of brain activity for every component.
Figure 4.11 shows 3D scalp EEG map for one of the 23 independent components’
scalp EEG maps for anxiety related EEG signal of Participant 3 (P3).
Figure 4.11
3D scalp EEG mapping for independent component of anxiety state
related data
Then, ICA-based EEG Energy Spectrum will be used to analyze the experiment
results. According to brain structure, biological basis of negative emotion and
50
4. Experimental Design
electrode placement, three types of EEG Energy Spectrum, which are Left Prefrontal
Energy Spectrum, Right Prefrontal Energy Spectrum and Anterior Temporal Energy
Spectrum, were defined to investigate the possible features of negative emotion
measurement by using EEG. The definitions are the following:
Left Prefrontal Energy Spectrum (LPES) is the total number of activation points in all
the independent components which have peak activation in Fp1 or F7. In order words,
independent components with peak activations at Fp1 or F7 would be considered to
have Left Prefrontal cortex activation. In such cases, for every independent
component which has activation in Fp1 or F7, the data tally for Left Prefrontal
Energy Spectrum for that participant would be increased by one if only Fp1 or F7 has
the peak activation and would be increased by two if both Fp1 and F7 have the peak
activation.
Right Prefrontal Energy Spectrum (RPES) is the total number of activation points in
all the independent components which have peak activation in Fp2 or F8. In order
words, independent components with peak activations at Fp2 or F8 would be
considered to have Right Prefrontal cortex activation. In such cases, for every
independent component which has activation in Fp2 or F8, the data tally for Right
Prefrontal Energy Spectrum for that participant would be increased by one if only
Fp2 or F8 has the peak activation and would be increased by two if both Fp2 and F8
have the peak activation.
51
4. Experimental Design
Anterior Temporal Energy Spectrum (ATES) is the total number of activation points
in all the independent components which have peak activation in the scalp sites X1,
X2, X3, X4, T3 and T4 or any combination of those six sites. In order words,
independent components with peak activations at X1, X2, X3, X4, T3 and T4 would
be considered to have temporal pole activation. In such cases, for every independent
component which has activation in X1, X2, X3, X4, T3 and T4, the data tally for
Anterior Temporal Energy Spectrum for that participant would be increased by the
number of activation point in these six points.
Because one independent component represent one independent source in the brain in
the aspect of EEG and the peak activation area represent the source energy, so the
LPES will indicate the left prefrontal cortex activation and the RPES will indicate the
right prefrontal cortex activation. Also, the ATES will indicate the temporal pole
activation. These three types of ICA-based EEG Energy Spectrum were used to
evaluate the negative emotion states, such as anxiety emotion, and the neutral
emotion state.
4.6
Support Vector Machine (SVM) Verification
Support Vector Machine (SVM) was used to verify the classable of EEG data
between anxiety state and neutral emotion state.
52
4. Experimental Design
4.6.1
SVM basic algorism
The best word to describe the EEG signal is complex. The EEG complexity originates
in the intricate neural system, which is almost a black-box to us. The complexity of
EEG signals requires some advanced signal processing methodology prior to any
brain activity identification. Therefore, to evaluate the EEG patterns related to
different emotion states, a standard artificial learning, two-class Support Vector
Machine was used. This machine learning method is widely used for classification
(pattern recognition) and regression models, and has been generally believed the best
statistical tool for classification and regression.
SVM are learning machines that can perform binary classification (pattern
recognition) and real valued function approximation (regression estimation) tasks
(Haykin 1999). SVM are generally competitive to (if not better than) Neural
Networks or other statistical pattern recognition techniques for solving pattern
recognition problems. It is also handy for solving regression problem, which is
convenient for continuous tracking fatigue. More importantly, SVM are showing high
performance in practical applications in recent studies. Therefore, SVM is chosen to
be used in this study. Figure 4.12, 4.13 and 4.14 show the good performance of SVM
as a binary classifier.
Consider two classes’ training vectors xi∈Rn, i=1, … , l, and the corresponding target
vector y∈{-1, 1}, SVM solves the following primal problem:
53
4. Experimental Design
l
1
min w T ⋅ w + C ∑ ξi
w ,b ,ξ 2
i =1
subject to yi (w T ⋅ φ ( xi ) + b) + ξi ≥ 1
ξi ≥ 0, i = 1, 2,..., l.
(4.3)
Its dual is
min
α
m
1 m
y
y
α
α
K
x
⋅
x
−
αi
(
)
∑ i j i j i j ∑
2 i , j =1
i =1
such that
l
∑ yα
i =1
i
i
=0
(4.4)
0 ≤ α i ≤ C, i = 1, 2,..., l.
The decision function is
l
sgn(∑ yiα i K (xi , x) + b)
i =1
Figure 4.12
(4.5).
Plot of two-class dataset
54
4. Experimental Design
Figure 4.13
Train-set plot and test-set plot
Figure 4.14 Resulting decision boundary of SVM and train-set or test-set data plot
The intuitive way to solve the multi-class classification is “one-against-one” approach.
In total of k(k-1)/2 classifiers are actually constructed and each one is trained using
data from two different classes. For training data from the ith and the jth classes, the
primal problem is:
55
4. Experimental Design
min
ij ij ij
w ,b ,ξ
l
1 ij T ij
(w ) ⋅ w + C ∑ ξtij
2
t =1
subject to ((w ij )T ⋅ φ (xi ) + bij ) ≥ 1 − ξtij , if xt in the ith class,
((w ij )T ⋅ φ (x i ) + bij ) ≤ −1 + ξtij , if xt in the jth class
(4.6)
ξtij ≥ 0, t = 1, 2,..., l.
w = vector of the separating hyperplane which is parameterized by (w,b)
x = position vectors of training data points
φ = function that maps input space to a high dimensional feature space
ξ = quadratic slack variable added as a measure of error.
C = parameter of trade off between fitting and error tolerance i.e. penalization of the
slack variable, ξ.
Since our objective is continuously monitoring emotion, the system’s output should
be able to track the subtle change of emotion in individuals. Therefore, the pattern
recognition should go for regression after essential features in relationship to emotion
are validated by means of multi-class classification. Given a set of available samples,
{(x1, z1), … , (xl, zl)}, such that zi∈R1 is a target value of input xi∈Rn, the standard
form of SVM for regression is:
l
l
1
min * w T ⋅ w + C ∑ ξi + C ∑ ξ i*
w ,b ,ξ ,ξ 2
i =1
i =1
subject to w T ⋅ φ (x i ) + b − zi ≤ ε − ξi ,
zi − w T ⋅ φ (x i ) − b ≤ ε + ξ i* ,
(4.7)
ξi , ξi* ≥ 0, t = 1, 2,..., l.
The corresponding dual problem is:
56
4. Experimental Design
l
l
1
min* (α − α* )T Q(α − α* ) + ε ∑ (α i − α i* ) + ∑ zi (α i − α i* )
α ,α 2
i =1
i =1
subject to
l
∑ (α
i =1
i
− α i* ) = 0,
(4.8)
0 ≤ α i , α i* ≤ C, i = 1, 2,..., l ,
where Qij=K(xi, xj)=φT(xi) φ(xj).
The resulting approximate function is:
l
∑ (−α
i =1
4.6.2
i
+α i* ) K ( xi , x) + b.
(4.9)
Data Labeling
Each subjects EEG data were labeled accordingly to the emotion states. In the
standard artificial learning, dual-class SVM was used to evaluate EEG patterns
related to the two different classes: Anxiety (AX) and Neutral Emotion (NE) States.
4.6.3
Feature Extraction
A fast Fourier transform (FFT) and Power Spectra Density (PSD) were performed on
the EEG data. Four features used were extracted from the power spectrum of the EEG
data. The frequency range was separated into four frequency bands, namely Delta
(1.5Hz~3.5Hz),
Theta
(3.5Hz~7.5Hz),
Alpha
(7.5Hz~12.5Hz)
and
Beta
(12.5Hz~25.0Hz). The four features were intended to characterize the power spectral
density of EEG data (Hao 1997). Their detailed definitions were as following:
57
4. Experimental Design
Feature 1: Dominant frequency
Every peak in the power spectrum corresponded to a peak frequency. The peak here
was defined as formed by two points. One of them was within the rising slope and the
other was within the falling slope, and they corresponded to amplitudes equal to half
the amplitude of the peak. These two frequencies formed a frequency band. This band
was called full width half maximum band of the peak. Among all the peaks in a
spectrum, the peak with the largest average power in its full width half maximum
band was called the dominant peak. The peak frequency corresponded to this
dominant peak was defined as dominant frequency. This feature was applied to each
frequency band.
Feature 2: Average power on the dominant peak
This was defined as the average power on the full width half maximum band of the
dominant peak.
Feature 3: Center of gravity frequencies
This parameter was defined as the frequencies that the power spectrum in the given
frequency range concentrate. In other words, we can consider this parameter as given
the normalized power spectrum as the probability, the mean of frequency. It was
58
4. Experimental Design
described by the following formula:
∑ P( f ) × f
C=
∑ P( f )
i
i
i
(4.10)
,
i
i
where P(fi) is the power at frequency fi.
Feature 4: Frequency variability
This feature was defined as the standard deviation of frequency given the power
spectrum as the probability distribution. It was given in the following formula:
⎡
⎛
⎞
⎢
⎜ ∑ P( fi ) × fi ⎟
⎠
⎢ P( f ) × f 2 − ⎝ i
i
i
⎢∑
P
(
f
)
i
∑i i
⎢
D=⎢
∑i P( fi )
⎢
⎢
⎢
⎢⎣
2
1
2
⎤
⎥
⎥
⎥
⎥
⎥ .
⎥
⎥
⎥
⎥⎦
(4.11)
The window used in estimating the power spectrum was 500 samples with the
sampling frequency 167 Hz, which was in total 3 seconds. Windows overlapped by
the time increment of 5 sample points. The dimension of the feature vector was 4
characteristics×4 frequency bands ×(19+4) channels = 368.
4.6.4
Training and testing SVM model
All the EEG datasets for different subjects and different emotion states were
separated equally into two parts, one was for training the SVM model (training data),
59
4. Experimental Design
and the other one was for testing the model (testing data). To achieve less bias, we
randomized the datasets for these two parts. The labeled training EEG data were fed
into SVM; an optimal C value as shown in Equation (4.6) was achieved. Therefore, a
dual-class SVM model was set up. Afterwards using the testing data to verify the
model, test accuracy was given as the output.
60
5. Results and Discussions
5
Results and Discussions
5.1 Effectiveness of ICA-based EEG Energy Spectrum
5.1.1
Anterior Temporal Energy Spectrum in negative emotion states vs. neutral
emotion state
Firstly, pain-induced anxiety state related EEG data was calculated by the Anterior
Temporal Energy Spectrum and compared with the ATES in neutral emotion state.
Figure 5.1 shows the ATES comparison between anxiety state and neutral emotion
state 1, in which the neutral emotion is induced by mentally mathematical calculation
and the anxiety emotion is induced by the electrical shocks. The results showed that
the averaged ATES in pain induced Anxiety state is 23.6 while it is 18.8 in Neutral
Emotion state 1, which means the averaged ATES in Anxiety State is increased by
25.5 percent in compared with the ATES in Neutral Emotion state 1.
Secondly, International Affective Picture System induced Unpleasant emotion related
EEG data was calculated by the Anterior Temporal Energy Spectrum and compared
with the ATES in neutral emotion state 2.
Figure 5.2 shows the ATES comparison between unpleasant emotion state and neutral
emotion state 2, in which the unpleasant emotion was induced by international
affective picture system and the neutral emotion state 2 was the baseline. The results
showed that the averaged ATES in IAPS induced negative emotion state is 18.2 while
61
5. Results and Discussions
it is 15.2 in neutral emotion state 2, which means the averaged ATES in IAPS
induced negative emotion state is increased by 19.7 percent in compared with the
ATES in neutral emotion state.
Figure 5.1
ATES comparison between anxiety state and neutral emotion state 1
Figure 5.2 ATES comparison between unpleasant emotion state and neutral emotion
state 2
62
5. Results and Discussions
However, there is no significant different between the ATES of positive emotion and
the ATES of neutral emotion in this research. This may because of the low arousal of
positive emotions by the International Affective Picture System or the failure of
inducing the positive emotions by the IAPS. This has been confirmed by the
questionnaire submitted by the participants after the experiment.
Our results showed that there is an obvious difference in ATES between anxiety state
or negative emotion state and neutral emotion state. Reiman et al. reported in a PET
study significant blood flow increases in the bilateral temporal poles during the
production of anticipatory anxiety (Reiman 1989). Other evidences showed that
patients with temporal pole epilepsy experience fear and anxiety, and the temporal
pole is associated with panic. Moreover, Yuri Masaoka also confirmed that the
temporal pole and Amygdala are associated with human anxiety, which means the
neuron groups in the temporal pole and Amygdala will be activated when the subject
is in anxiety state. (Masaoka 2000) Thus, negative emotions, such as anxiety, can be
considered as the result of sub-neuron groups’ activation of temporal pole and
Amygdala, which will appear in peak electrical potentials at specific locations on the
scalp. By counting the number of these peak electrical potentials, the intensity of
neuron activation of temporal pole and Amygdala can be determined. This forms the
principle of the anterior temporal Energy Spectrum of anxiety.
From the brain anatomy, the anterior temporal region is the one which covers the
temporal pole and Amygdala. So the ATES was calculated and the result is consistent
63
5. Results and Discussions
with all literatures results, indicates that negative emotions, especially anxiety, causes
discernible differences in EEG data and these differences are detectable using EEG.
Furthermore, the ATES in the pain-induced anxiety is larger than ATES in the
IAPS-induced negative emotion, for which one of the possible reasons is the low
arousal of emotions by the static pictures system.
5.1.2
Asymmetry of Prefrontal Energy Spectrum in negative emotion states vs.
control emotion state
The asymmetry of frontal power spectrum is illustrated by the comparison between
the averaged Left and Right Prefrontal Energy Spectrum. Figure 5.3 shows the
averaged LFPS in anxiety state is 10.6, in compared with 9.6 in neutral emotion state,
while averaged RFPS is 12.6 in anxiety state, in compared with only 8.6 in neutral
emotion state, which means the ratio of LFPS and RFPS decrease a lot in anxiety
state in compared with in neutral state.
Then, IAPS induced negative emotion related EEG data and neutral emotion related
EEG data were analyzed and compared by the Left Prefrontal Energy Spectrum and
Right Prefrontal Energy Spectrum.
Figure 5.4 shows the averaged LFPS in unpleasant state is 14.6, in compared with
10.8 in neutral emotion state, while averaged RFPS is 18 in anxiety state, in
compared with only 13 in neutral emotion state, which means the ratio of LFPS and
64
5. Results and Discussions
RFPS decrease a lot in unpleasant emotion state in compared with in neutral emotion
state.
Figure 5.3
Figure 5.4
Prefrontal Energy Spectrum in anxiety state and neutral state
Prefrontal Energy Spectrum in negative emotion state and neutral
emotion state
Using EEG to study brain asymmetry in humans, researchers have recently made
many discoveries suggesting that individual differences in electrical activity between
the two brain hemispheres can be used to predict emotional responses to various
stimuli. On the basis of a large body of both human and animal studies, Davidson and
65
5. Results and Discussions
his colleagues (Davidson 2003) have proposed that greater left-sided dorsolateral
activity may be associated with approach-related, goal-directed action planning,
whereas on a lesser level of consensus, based on neuron-imaging studies of spatial
working memory, they suggested that activation of right lateral prefrontal cortex
during withdrawal-related emotion may be associated with threat-related vigilance.
Davidson also reported that positive and negative affective states shift the asymmetry
in prefrontal brain electrical activity in lawful ways. For example, film-induced
negative affect i.e fear/anxiety increases relative right-sided prefrontal cortex
activation, whereas induced positive affect elicits an opposite pattern of asymmetric
activation. The results from Figure 5.3 are mostly consistent with Davidson’s findings;
with the exception that positive affect was not induced for the current study. For all
participants, the percentage of right prefrontal cortex activations in AX averaged
18.8% higher than that of left prefrontal cortex activation, implying that under anxiety
states; right prefrontal cortex activity is invariably heightened when compared to the
control (NE).
Recent neuron-imaging findings have demonstrated inverse relationships between
activity in the Amygdala and regions of prefrontal cortex. One particular study using
PET indicated that in normal subjects, glucose metabolism in left medial and lateral
PFC is inversely associated with glucose metabolic rate in the Amygdala. It follows
that subjects with greater relative right-sided prefrontal metabolism have higher
metabolic activity in their Amygdala. Superimposed with findings from the current
66
5. Results and Discussions
study it can be inferred that lower ratio of LPES and RPES in AX compared to NE is
explained by the positive correlation of right PFC activity with Amygdala activity.
Therefore, electrical activity in the right PFC is found to be an indirect measure of the
level of activity at the Amygdala. One possible neuron-physiological explanation for
this is that the prefrontal cortex has extensive anatomical connections with the limbic
structures like the Amygdala. It implies that the Amygdala is indirectly implicated
with the prefrontal cortex in this complex neural circuitry of negative affect.
Other findings have supported that the prefrontal cortex is part of a neural mechanism
that regulates emotional responses mediated by the Amygdala through conscious
evaluation and appraisal. In the pain-induced anxiety stage, participants of the
experiment were blindfolded and were unintentionally forced to rely on other cues
such as sound and subtle changes in airflow near the skin, to predict when the electric
shock would occur. When a cue emerges, the participant may feel a sudden wave of
anxiety temporarily. However, the feeling of anxiety dies down upon recognizing that
it was a false alarm i.e. the shock did not strike then. The state of anxiety does not
persist because there is a cognitive, conscious evaluation of the situation that involves
the prefrontal cortex in the neural modulation of negative emotion. Negative emotion
regulation is implied only in states of anxiety and not in emotionless or positive
affective states because the participant is not experiencing anything negative in the
first place. Thus, the results are explained even further by the role in which the
prefrontal cortex plays in regulating negative emotional responses from the Amygdala
through indirect inhibitory connections.
67
5. Results and Discussions
Collectively, the findings regarding decreased ratio of LPES and RPES in AX
compared to NE indicates that EEG can detect anxiety through prefrontal power
spectrum. This further substantiates the point that negative emotions, especially
anxiety, causes discernible differences in EEG data and these differences are
detectable using EEG.
5.1.3
Validation of Experiment design
In our results, the positive emotions induced by IAPS can not be differentiated from
baseline or neutral emotions induced by IAPS. This is because of the difficult in
inducing positive emotion in the laboratory condition, which has been pointed out in
the literatures. And also this has been confirmed by the questionnaire from the
participants after the experiments. In the questionnaire, the subjects pointed out that
they were experiencing much more in the negative emotion inducing process than in
the positive emotion inducing process.
Furthermore, using IAPS to induce negative emotion has been verified by many other
researchers, and their results have shown that IAPS has more effect in inducing
negative emotions than in inducing positive emotions, which is consistent with our
results.
68
5. Results and Discussions
For the pain-induced anxiety, it could be beyond the comprehension of some as to
why this study had performed experiments using induced anxiety with pain. Also,
some skeptics may argue that the stimulus of this study i.e. anticipating an electrical
shock; may not be sufficient to induce a state of anxiety in the study participant.
In a study on anxiety related respiratory potentials, the temporal poles and the
Amygdala showed increased levels of oxygen consumption activity when the
participants in the experiment were subjected to anxiety inducing stimuli. (Masaoka
2000) The stimulus used in this experiment was an electric pulse that stung the
forefinger of each experiment participant. Anxiety was self-reported after the
experiment was completed and said to occur during periods where the participants
anticipated the electric stimulus (Masaoka 2000). Since both experimental results and
self report implicated a state of anxiety, it is with a high degree of certainty that this
experimental method was effective for Masaoka’s study. As the anxiety inducing
methods used in both studies are similar, the experimental data acquired is safely
assumed to contain states of anxiety.
Since each participant has five trails to be induced anxiety, the relationship between
the induced anxiety and the experiment runs has been investigated. Figure 5.5 shows
the trend of the averaged asymmetry of frontal energy spectrum changing with the
experiment trails in anxiety state and neutral emotion state.
69
5. Results and Discussions
In Figure 5.5, the ratio of the LPES and RPES in anxiety state increased with the
experiment runs. Moreover, the ratio of the LPES and RPES in neutral emotion state
is lower than that in anxiety emotion in the first 4 trails, but in the fifth trail, the result
is just the opposite. The possible reason is that the participant may become more and
more getting used to these electrical shocks, thus the induced effect will be decrease,
even there is no anxiety in the fifth trail.
Figure 5.5 Validation of experiment design
The horizontal axis is the experiment trails and the vertical axis is the ratio of LPES
and RPES, which indicates the asymmetry of frontal energy spectrum. In this figure,
the dotted line represented the asymmetry of frontal energy spectrum in the neutral
emotion state, while the real line stood for the asymmetry of frontal energy spectrum
in the anxiety state.
For the neutral emotion state, the participant was requested to do the mental
mathematical calculation according to the experiment protocol; therefore, the long
time mental mathematical calculation could lead the participant to produce some
other negative affect, such as slightly dysphoria, slightly depression. All these have
been confirmed by the questionnaire for all participants after the experiments. So the
experiment should not be repeated too many times on the same participant in one time
slot.
70
5. Results and Discussions
5.1.4
SVM Verification of the EEG data
The level of accuracy is calculated to be 86.4% by using default C value of 1.0. As
we know, C is a parameter of the trade off between fitting and error tolerance.
However, the default value is not the best value to be used during SVM prediction of
the two classed (AX and NE). An iterative train and test method that is analogous to
“tuning” may be used such that the C parameter is optimized. To achieve this, each
raw EEG data set used to train the SVM is trained and tested against the other data
sets which are also involved in the SVM training. An optimal C value is obtained
through this training, which will result in a heightened accuracy rate when the SVM
tests and predicts EEG data as AX or NE. Figure 5.6 shows the results from the
optimization of C parameter when testing and training hand-picked values from 2-4 to
23. The C value with the highest accuracy rate is the optimal C value.
Accuracy Rate (%)
87
86.6299
86.5
86.4063
86.1678
86
85.7356
85.5
85
84.5
84.4165
84
83.701
83.5
0
Figure 5.6
2
4 C Value
6
8
10
Relationship between Training Accuracy Rate and C value of SVM
during optimization
71
5. Results and Discussions
The optimal C is found to be C=4.0. Using this new optimal C parameter in the
SVM’s prediction of EEG data, the results are shown in Table 5.1:
Table 5.1 SVM prediction result with optimal C
Parameter
Value
Accuracy rate
86.9429%
Mean squared error
0.130571
Squared correlation coefficient 0.545955
Therefore, instead of the accuracy rate of 86.4% procured by the default C parameter
of 1.0, the optimal C parameter of 4.0 found the accuracy rate to be 86.9%.
The SVM prediction accuracy results show that there are obvious differences between
negative emotion state and neutral emotion state in the aspect of EEG data, which
have been confirmed by our experiment results.
72
6. Conclusions
6 Conclusions
6.1 Conclusions
This study is mainly to develop the novel signal processing methodology and pattern
recognition system, which can be used to detect and identify subtle changes in the
EEG signal in relationship to negative emotions of individuals through some
measurable characteristics.
6.1.1
ICA-based EEG Energy Spectrum has been proposed
The ICA based EEG energy spectrum at a particular location is defined by
the number of ICA components with the peak potential at the location, in which
each ICA component corresponds to a specific neuronal activation in the brain.
The energy spectrum has been applied to the negative emotions, such as
anxiety, measurement by counting the energy spectrum at the prefrontal and
anterior temporal regions.
The experimental results showed that the anterior temporal energy spectrum
increased significantly and the ratio of right prefrontal energy spectrum to left
prefrontal energy spectrum increases significantly from Neutral Emotion mode
to Negative Emotion mode.
73
6. Conclusions
6.1.2
Negative emotions, especially anxiety, causes discernible differences in EEG
data in compared with neutral emotion and these differences are detectable
using EEG
A series experiments have been designed for the negative emotion detection
and experiment results have been analyzed by ICA based EEG Energy
Spectrum and verified by SVM. The results have shown that the experimental
protocol is useful for the negative emotion measurement or detection.
Negative emotions and neutral emotion have shown significant differences
by using ICA based EEG Energy Spectrum analysis, including the anterior
temporal energy spectrum and asymmetry of prefrontal energy spectrum.
The dual-class SVM prediction has achieved very high accuracy, which
substantiate that there are obvious differences between negative emotion state
and neutral emotion state in the aspect of EEG data
Our results have shown that one type of specific emotion stimulus should not
be put on the subjects for the long time; otherwise, the same stimulus will
produce the opposite effect. For example, mental mathematical calculation was
designed to induce the neutral emotion, but the long time of mental
mathematical calculation would induce the negative emotions, such as
abhorring. Also, the fear of pain was designed to induce the negative emotions,
such as anxiety, however, the long term of the pain will let the subject get used
to this feeling and the induced negative emotion would be weakened.
74
6. Conclusions
6.2 Recommendations for Future Work
Although the proposed method has achieved the primary objective of negative
emotion measurement using EEG and SVM, improvements can be made to make this
method more accurate and reliable. Directions in which this work could be further
explored and enhanced are as follows:
1. Further consider and improve the experiment design. Our limitation of this study
lies in the emotion stimulus. The positive emotion induced by IAPS was unsuccessful
in the experiments. So other types of stimulus for positive emotion should be
considered. Also, the stimulus for negative emotion should also be standardized. Such
as, the electrical pulse should be exerted by the clinical nerve conduction tester
capable of generating mild electrical shock, by which the pain induced anxiety degree
can be controllable.
2. Recruit a larger population samples and include wider range. Not all subjects
demonstrated the same set of physiological characteristics because of individual
differences such as age, gender, or the different ability to control emotion. And the
detection and prediction accuracy could be increase when a larger sample of testing
data was used. Hence future experimentations should increase the sample size and
include a wider range such as age and races.
75
6. Conclusions
3. New EEG machine should be used in the future experiments. The new EEG
machine should be stable and have large frequency bandwidth. Especially, this new
EEG system should have enough input channels for the possible appended electrodes.
4. In this study, only the dual SVM prediction has been conducted, thus in future
experiments, the multi emotion states should be considered together and the
multi-SVM prediction should be considered.
76
References
REFERENCES
A, S. M. (1977). "Ear differences in evaluating emotional tones of voice and verbal
content." Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance(3):
75-82.
Ahern, G. L. (1979). "Differential lateralization for positive versus negative emotion."
Neuropsychologia(17): 693-698.
Benar, C. (2003). "Quality of EEG in simutaneous EEG-fMRI for epilepsy." Clinical
Neurophysiology 114: 569-580.
Berger, H. (1929). "Üer das Elektrenkephalogramm des Menschen (On the EEG in
humans)." Arch. Psychiatr. Nervenkr 87: 527-570.
Bickmore, T. W. (2004). "Unspoken Rules of Spoken Interaction." Communications
of the ACM 47(4): 38-44.
Bloom, B. S. (1984). "The 2 Sigma Problem: The Search for Method of Group
Instruction as Effective as One-to-One Tutoring." Educational Researcher 13(6):
4-16.
Bradley, M. M., Codispoti, M., Cuthbert, B. N., & Lang, P. J (2001). "Emotion and
motivation: I. Defensive and appetitive reactions in picture processing." Emotion 1:
276-298.
Bradley, M. M., Codispoti, M., Sabatinelli, D., & Lang, P. J (2001). "Emotion and
motivatioin II: Sex differences in picture processing." Emotion 1: 300-319.
Cacioppo, J. T. (1993). "If attitudes affect how stimuli are processed, should they not
affect the event-related brain potential?" Psychological Science(4): 108-112.
Cacioppo, J. T. (1994). "Bioelectrical echoes from evaluative categorization: I. A late
positive brain potential that varies as a function of trait negativity and extremity."
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology(67): 115-125.
Cacioppo, J. T. (1994). "Relationships between attitudes and evaluative space: A
critical review, with emphasis on the separability of positive and negative substrates."
Psychological Bulletin(115): 401-423.
Cacioppo, J. T. (1996). "Attitudes to the right: Evaluative processing is associated
with lateralized late positive event-related brain potentials." Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin(22): 1205-1219.
77
References
Cannon, W. B. (1927). "The James-Lange theory of emotions: A Critical
Examination and an Alternative Theory." American Journal of Psychology 39:
115-124.
Carmon, A. (1973). "Ear asymmetry in perception of emotional non-verbal stimuli."
ACTA PSYCHOLOGICA (37): 351-357.
Coleman-Mesches K, M. J. (1995). "Differential effects of pretraining inactivation of
the right or left amygdala on retention of inhibitory avoidance training." Behavioral
Neuroscience(109): 642-647.
Common, P. (1994). "Independent component analysis-A new concept?" Signal
processing 36: 287-314.
D.Goleman (1995). "Emotional Intelligence." New York: Bantam Books.
Davidson, R. (1992). "Emotion and affective style: Hemispheric substrates." Psychol.
Sci.(3): 39-43.
Davidson, R. J. (1976). "Patterns of cerebral lateralization during cardiac biofeedback
versus the self-regulation of emotion: sex differences." Psychophysiology(13): 62-68.
Davidson, R. J. (1995). "Cerebral asymmetry, emotion and affective style, in Brain
Asymmetry " MIT Press: 361-387.
Davidson, R. J. (2001). "Anxiety and affective style: role of prefrontal cortex and
amygdala." Journal of Biological Psychiatry 51(1): 68-80.
Davidson, R. J. (2003). "Approach/withdrawal and cerebral asymmetry: Emotional
expression and brain physiology." International Journal Personality Social
Psychology(58): 330-341.
Davidson, R. J. (2003). "Parsing the subcomponents of emotion and disorders of
emotion: perspectives from affective neuroscience." Handbook of Affective Sciences.
Oxford University Press, New York: 8-24.
Davidson, R. J. (2004). "What does the prefrontal cortex "do" in affect: perspectives
on frontal EEG asymmetry research." Biological Psychology 67(12): 219-233.
Davis, W. J., Rahman, M. A., Smith, L. J., Burns, A., Senecal, L.,et al (1995).
"Properties of the human affect induced by static color slides (IAPS): Dimensional,
categorical and electromyographic analysis."
Dimond, S. J. (1977). "Emotion response to films shown to the right or left
hemisphere of the brain measured by heart rate." ACTA PSYCHOLOGICA(41):
255-260.
78
References
Donchin, E. (1988). "Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating?"
Behavioral and Brain Sciences(11): 357-427.
Ekman, P. (1972). "Emotion in the Human Face: Guildelines for Research and an
Integration of Findings." New York: Pergamon Press.
Enns, J. T. (1991). "Preattentive recovery of threedimensional orientation from line
drawings." Psychological Review(98): 335-351.
Fotinea, S.-E. (2003). "Emotion in Speech: Towards an Integration of Linguistic,
Paralinguistic, and Psychological Analysis." ICANN: 1125-1132.
Frijda, N. H., Kuipers, P., & ter Schure, E (1989). "Relations among emotion,
appraisal,and emotional action readiness." Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology 57(2): 212-228.
Gainotti, G. (1972). "Emotional behavior and hemispheric side of lesion." Cortex(8):
41-55.
Gardner, H. (1975). "Comprehension and appreciation of humorous material
following brain damage." Brain(98): 399-412.
Gray, J. A. (1994). "Three fundamental emotion systems, in The Nature of Emotion:
Fundamental Questions." Oxford University Press: 243-247.
Hao, Q. a. G., J (1997). "A patient-specific algorithm for the detection of seizure
onset in long-term EEG monitoring: possible use as a warning device." IEEE
transactions on medical engineering 44: 115-122.
Hare, T. A. (2005). "Contributions of Amygdala and Striatal Activity in Emotion
Regulation." Journal of Biological Psychiatry(57): 624-632.
Hariri, A. R. (2003). "Neocortical Modulation of the Amygdala Response to Fearful
Stimuli." Journal of Biological Psychiatry(53): 494-501.
Haykin, S. (1999). "Neural Networks." New Jersey: Prentice-Hall.
He, Z. J. (1992). "Surface versus features in visual search." Nature(359): 231-233.
Herault, C. J. a. J. (1991). "Blind separation of sources, part I: An adaptive algorithm
based on neuromimetic architecture." Signal processing 24: 1-10.
Hyvarinen, A. (1999). "Fast and robust fixed-point algorithms for independent
component analysis." IEEE transactions on neural networks 10: 626-634.
79
References
Hyvarinen, A. (2000). "Independent component
applications." Neural Networks 13: 411-430.
analysis:
algorithms
and
Isen, A. M. (1987). "Positive affect facilitates creative problem solving." Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology 52(6): 1122-1131.
J.LeDoux (1996). "The Emotional Brain." New York: Simon & Schuster.
James, W. (1890). "The Principles of Psychology." New York: Holt.
Jongh, A. D. (2001). "The localization of spontatneous brain activity: first results in
patients with cerebral tumors." Clinical Neurophysiology 112: 378-385.
Kahneman, D. (1973). "Attention and Effort." 28-49.
Kaiser, S. W., T (1996). "Situated emotional problemsolving in interactive
computergames." Proceedings of the VIIIth Conference of ISRE.
Kandel E. R., S. J. H., Jessell T. M (2000). "Principles of Neural Science." New York:
McGraw-Hill.
Karhunen, J. (1997). "A class of neural networks for independent component
analysis." IEEE transactions on neural networks 8.
Klein, J. (2002). "This Computer Responds to User Frustration: Theory, Design, and
Results." Interacting with Computers(14): 119-140.
L, G. (1985). "Differential Lateralization for positive and negative emotion in the
human brain: EEG Spectral Analysis." Neuropsychologia(23): 745-755.
Lang, P. J., & Greenwald, M. K (1988). "The international affective picture system
standardization procedure and initial group results for affective judgments: Technical
reports 1A & 1B." Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida.
Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N (1998). "Emotion, motivation, and
anxiety: Brain mechanisms and psychophysiology." Biological Psychiatry 44:
1248-1263.
Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N (2005). "The international affective
picture system (IAPS): Technical manual and affective ratings." Center for Research
in Psychophysiology, University of Florida.
Lang, P. J., Bradley, M.M. and Cuthbert, B.N (1990). "Emotion, attention and the
startle reflex." Psychology Review(97): 377-398.
Lautin, A. (2001). "The Limbic Brain." New York: Academic Press.
80
References
Lehnertz, K. (2001). "Nonlinear EEG analysis in epilepsy: its possible use for
interictal focus localization, seizure anticipation, and prevention." Clinical
Neurophysiology 18.
Lewis, V. E. (1989). "Mood-congruent vs. Mood-state-dependent Learning:
Implications for a View of Emotion." Mood and Memory: Theory, Research, and
Applications: 157-171.
Maclean, P. (1952). "Some psychiatric implications of physiological studies on
frontotemporal portion of limbic system (visceral brain)." Electroencephalogr Clin
Neurophysiol 4(4): 407-418.
Maren, S. (2001). "Neurobiology of Pavlovian fear conditioning." Annual Review of
Neuroscience 24: 897-931.
Masaoka, Y. (2000). "The source generator of respiratory-related anxiety potential in
the human brain." Neuroscience Letters (283): 21-24.
Mauro, R., Sato, K., & Tucker, J. (1992). "The role of appraisal in human emotions:
A cross-cultural study." Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 62: 301-317.
Mayer, P. S. a. J. D. (1990). "Emotional Intelligence." Imagination, Cognition and
Personality 9(3): 185-211.
Miles, J. D. (1996). "Using artificial neural networks to classify mental tasks."
Biomedical Engineering, University of Southern California.
Mishra, P. (2004). "Etiquette and the Design of Educational Technology."
Communications of the ACM 47(4): 45-49.
Müller, M. M., Keil, A., Gruber, T., Elbert, T (1999). "Processing affective pictures
modulates right-hemispheric gamma band EEG activity." Clinical Neurophysiology
110: 1913-1920.
Myers, D. G. (2004). "Psychology." Worth Publishers.
O’Shaughnessy, J. (1992). "Explaining Buyer Behavior." Oxford University
Press(Central Concept and Philosophy of Science Issues): P178.
Oldfield, R. C. (1971). "The assessment and analysis of handednessL The Edinburgh
Inventory." Neuropsycholoogia 9: 97-113.
Olds, J., Milner, P (1954). "Positive reinforcement produced by electrical stimulation
of septal area and other regions of rat brain." Journal of Comp. Physiolo. Psycholo.
47: 419-427.
81
References
Papez, J. W. (1937). "A proposed mechanism of emotion." Arch. Neurol Psychiatry
38: 725-743.
Preece, J. (1994). "Human-Computer Interaction." New York: Addison-Wesley.
Reiman, E. M., Fusselman, M.J., Fox, P.T. and Raichle, M.E. (1989).
"Neuroanatomical correlates of anticipatory anxiety." Science 243: 1071-1074.
Salovey, P. (1990). "Emotional Intelligence." Imagination, Cognition and Personality
9(3): 185-211.
Schachter, S. (1971). "Emotion, Obesity and Crime." New York: Academic Press.
Schmidt, L. A. (2002). "Frontal brain electrical activity (EEG) distinguishes valence
and intensity of musical emotions." Cognition & Emotion(15): 487-500.
Smilek, D. (2000). "Does unattended information facilitate change detection?"
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance(26):
480-487.
Suzuki, S. (1995). "Facial organization blocks access to low-level features: An object
inferiority effect." Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception &
Performance(21): 901-913.
Thakor, N. V. (2004). "Advances in quantitative electroencephalogram analysis
methods." Annual Review of Biomedical Engineering 6: 1-43.
Thayerb, J. F. B. a. J. F. (2003). "Heart rate response is longer
emotions than after positive emotions."
after
negative
Van, L. W. S. (1950). "EEG in metastatic brain tumour before, during and after
radiation treatment." Electroencephalography And Clinical Neurophysiology 2:
331-332.
van Reekum, C. M., Johnstone, T., Banse, R., Etter, A., Wehrle, T., & Scherer, K. R
(2004). "Psychophysiological responses to appraisal dimensions in a computer
game." Cognition and Emotion 18: 663-688.
Vigario, R. N. (1997). "Extraction of ocular artefacts from EEG using independent
component analysis." Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 103:
395-404.
Wang, Q. (1994). "Familiarity and pop-out in visual search." Perception &
Psychophysics(56): 495-500.
82
References
Zald DH, L. J., Fluegel KW, Pardo JW (1998). "Aversive gustatory stimulation
activates limbic circuits in humans." Brain(121): 1143-1154.
Zald DH, P. J. (1997). "Emotion, olfaction and the human amygdala: Amygdala
activation during aversive olfactory stimulation." Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 94:
4119-4124.
83
[...]... because of this ineffectiveness of this signal processing method Thus, considered the complexity of EEG signals, Independent Component Analysis (ICA) has been investigated as well as the biological basis of emotion and brain structure, a novel ICA- based EEG Energy Spectrum was proposed and used to evaluate some negative emotions, such as anxiety 17 3 ICA- based EEG Energy Spectrum 3 ICA- based EEG Energy Spectrum. .. Figure 3.8 is one example of scalp EEG map, which indicates a special activation pattern in left anterior temporal region Figure 3.7 Figure 3.8 Color scaling algorism Example of Scalp EEG map 26 3 ICA- based EEG Energy Spectrum 3.4 ICA- based EEG Energy Spectrum For Scalp EEG maps of ICA result, it can be both 2D scalp EEG map and 3D scalp EEG map (Figure 3.9) So after ICA and scalp EEG mapping, all the independent... EEG Energy Spectrum This chapter describes the biological basis of ICA- based EEG Energy Spectrum, as well as the principle of ICA- based EEG Energy Spectrum, which includes Independent Component Analysis, Scalp EEG mapping, and ICA- based EEG Energy Spectrum calculation 3.1 Biological Basis As we know, different kinds of brain activities are the result of some neuron groups firing in the certain time sequence... the critical electrodes placement design for the negative emotion detection by using EEG; 3) To analyze the experiment results for the effectiveness of this physical quantity; 4) To verify the results for this physical quantity by using Support Vector Machine (SVM) 6 2 Literature Review 2 LITERATURE REVIEW 2.1 Traditional Technologies in emotion detection Traditional technologies in emotion detection. .. exhibited a significant increase in power for positive and negative valence relative to neutral stimuli for γ-40 power compared to the neutral condition, and also no statistically significant effect was found for alpha activity in anxiety state, indicating no sensitivity of alpha de-synchronization All these arguments weaken the possibility of negative emotion detection by electrical asymmetry Secondly,... calculation of ICA- based EEG Energy Spectrum consists of four steps: Independent Component Analysis, Scalp EEG Mapping, Brain Activity Classification and Statistical Analysis 3.2 Independent Component Analysis (ICA) Independent component analysis (ICA) is a computational method for separating a multivariate signal into additive subcomponents supposing the mutual statistical independence of the non-Gaussian... new physical quantity, which is named ICA- based EEG Energy Spectrum, for the features in identifying subtle changes in the EEG signal in relationship to negative emotions Under this 5 1 Introduction primary objective, the detailed sub-objectives are the following: 1) To establish the analysis of this physical quantity; 2) To establish the experiments for verifying the effectiveness of this physical quantity,... intensity of neuron groups’ activation related to some brain activity can be determined Here the intensity of neuron groups’ firing represents the neurons activation energy Therefore, the specific brain activity can be monitored or measured by the number of the “peak” electrical potentials appearing in the specific locations on the scalp, and this forms the basis of ICA- based EEG Energy Spectrum Under... electrical potentials to appear at specific locations on the scalp Figure 3.1 shows some brain activities with different neurons firing pattern Figure 3.1 Some Brain Activities 18 3 ICA- based EEG Energy Spectrum For simplification, each of this activated neuron group can be viewed as one electrical source and all the electrical sources are independent on each other Thus, by summarizing the peak electrical... components from their mixtures by using ICA is non-Gaussianity Intuitively speaking, maximizing the norm of this kurtosis leads to the separation of one non-Gaussian source from the observed mixtures In our algorithm, non-Gaussianity is measured by the classical fourth-order cumulant or kurtosis Consider y = wTv, with ||w|| = 1, kurtosis is calculated by 21 3 ICA- based EEG Energy Spectrum kurt (y ) = E{(y ... basis of emotion and brain structure, a novel ICA- based EEG Energy Spectrum was proposed and used to evaluate some negative emotions, such as anxiety 17 ICA- based EEG Energy Spectrum ICA- based EEG. .. 27 ICA- based EEG Energy Spectrum Figure 3.10 Four direction view of 3D scalp EEG mapping for the ICA result Figure 3.11 classification of 3D scalp EEG mapping for the ICA results 28 ICA- based EEG. .. this forms the basis of ICA- based EEG Energy Spectrum Under this principle, the calculation of ICA- based EEG Energy Spectrum consists of four steps: Independent Component Analysis, Scalp EEG Mapping,