Tài liệu hạn chế xem trước, để xem đầy đủ mời bạn chọn Tải xuống
1
/ 93 trang
THÔNG TIN TÀI LIỆU
Thông tin cơ bản
Định dạng
Số trang
93
Dung lượng
361,4 KB
Nội dung
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
Differences between heterosexual and lesbian women in
acceptability and risk perceptions associated with sexual
permissiveness
In many countries, homosexuality is a crime with
varying degrees of severity of punishment. In Saudi Arabia and
Iran, it is punishable by death. In Jamaica, those convicted are
sentenced to 10 years hard labor. In Malaysia, a jail sentence
awaits. The United Nations General Assembly, which heard two
opposing statements on lesbian, gay, bisexual, transsexual, and
intersex (LGBTI) rights on December 18, 2008, shed some light on
prevalent global sentiment. It was argued that discrimination,
violence, criminal sanctions, and abuse based on sexual or gender
identity violate LGBTI human rights. Navanethem Pillay, the
United Nations High Commissioner for human rights, commented that
laws against homosexuality “are increasingly becoming recognized
as anachronistic and as inconsistent both with international law
and with traditional values of dignity, inclusion and respect for
all” (Macfarquhar, 2008). The response to LGBTI rights among
member countries, however, was mixed. Sixty-six nations supported
such rights, 57 were against them, and 69 abstained. However, on
closer examination, the picture for Asia was more consistent. In
Asia, only Armenia, Japan, Nepal, and Timor-Leste supported LGBTI
rights. Indonesia, Malaysia, and Brunei opposed. The rest of the
South East Asian countries, including Singapore, abstained.
Therefore, by their abstinence, the majority of countries in Asia
showed a sense of ambivalence towards LGBTI rights.
The stance taken by Singapore in the recent UN General
Assembly is reflective of its population’s mixed response to the
gay and lesbian community. An unsuccessful attempt to
1
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
decriminalize sex acts between two men (section 377A in the penal
code) in 2007 brought this issue to the forefront. Under section
377A, it is stated, “penetration is sufficient to constitute the
carnal intercourse necessary to the offence described in this
section”. Opponents of the repeal of section 377A had spoken
against the decriminalization of sex between men. In response to
the fierce debate on both sides, the government opted to remain
with the status quo (Ramesh, 2007, September 22). Prime Minister
Lee Hsien Loong said, “the tone of society, the public, and
society as a whole, should be really set by the heterosexuals and
that’s the way many Singaporeans feel… what people do in private
is their own business; in public, certain norms apply” (Ramesh,
2007, September 22). Therefore, sex acts between males continue
to be criminalized, even though there remain strong feelings
among both camps.
Unlike their gay counterparts, lesbian women were left out
of the penal code and newspaper articles related to the acquired
immune deficiency syndrome (AIDS). This did not mean, however,
that they were spared from being portrayed in a negative light.
Some religious groups, for instance, warned that living a lesbian
lifestyle would end in misery because of greater promiscuity in
the lesbian community (e.g., Harley, 2003). In a brochure
entitled “Straight Talk”, given out to secondary school students
in Singapore, significant numbers of lesbian women were said to
exchange sex for money and drugs (Harley, 2003). It was also
written that “lesbian relationships are equally unhealthy and
just as life-threatening as gay male relationships”. The sexual
health consequences of lesbian activities were emphasized.
2
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
Promiscuity has been defined as irresponsible sexuality,
such as having unprotected sex with multiple sexual partners,
with adverse health consequences such as sexually transmitted
infections (STIs) and unwanted pregnancy (Gershman, 1997; Markey
& Markey, 2007; Widom & Kuhms, 1996). Sexual permissiveness
appears to cover any sexual behavior that falls outside of a
committed marriage. Examples of sexual permissiveness are onenight stands and having threesomes. Certain behaviors are more
ambiguous however. For instance, some people perceive premarital
sex within committed dating partners as acceptable, but there are
others who view the same as sexual permissiveness. Therefore,
promiscuity appears to be broadly captured by the concept of
sexual permissiveness coupled with irresponsibility and adverse
health consequences. The chief emphasis of many current safe sex
campaigns is on responsible sex. While safe sex campaigners
advocate being faithful to one sexual partner, their work among
female sex workers stresses protective measures so that adverse
consequences can be avoided. Thus, while promiscuity includes
sexual permissiveness, sexual permissiveness can be meaningfully
distinguished from promiscuity in that the latter implies both
irresponsibility and adverse health consequences, while sexual
permissiveness can exist with responsibility (i.e., having
multiple partners but always practicing safe sex) that eliminates
or at least minimizes adverse health consequences.
Are lesbian women sexually permissive? While there is a
paucity of research studies on sexual behaviors and attitudes of
lesbian women in Asia, research conducted outside of Asian
countries suggests that lesbian women can be sexually permissive.
In Morrow and Allsworth’s (2000) survey study on sexual risks in
3
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
lesbian women and bisexual women, 13% of their lesbian sample
(total lesbian sample size was 436) were then sexually inactive,
70% had a single sexual partner, and 17% had multiple partners.
Of those who had been sexually active in the previous year, 30%
of their lesbian participants ever had multiple female sexual
partners. In Montcalm and Myers’ (2000) study, lesbian
participants reported between none to 110 sexual partners in the
previous year, with 72% having had only one sexual partner.
Therefore, while the majority of lesbian women in these two
samples prefer monogamy, approximately a quarter of them do
engage in sex with multiple partners.
Perceived risk influences the use of safe sex methods.
Lesbian women who had a history of STIs, had a friend with human
immunodeficiency virus (HIV), or perceived greater risk for the
self were more likely to be concerned about STIs and practice
safe sex (Diamant, Lever & Schuster, 2000; Montcalm & Myer,
2000). However, it is likely that Singaporean lesbian women think
that they are impervious to the effects of STIs. Newspapers
frequently allude to HIV and AIDS as a gay man’s illness (e.g.,
Wong, 2008). Reporting on infected heterosexuals tends to be
male-centric, such as a focus on males who participate in the
Batam (Indonesia) sex trade. In such instances, married women are
portrayed as victims, being infected as a result of and without
knowledge of their husbands’ extramarital sexual stints. HIV and
AIDS are not typically associated with lesbian women in local
news reporting, possibly contributing to the perception that
lesbian women are invulnerable. This perception, where many
lesbian women see themselves as “safe” from STIs, has been termed
“lesbian immunity” (Montcalm & Myer, 2000), which is a perception
4
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
of low or no sexual risk when the sexual partner is a fellow
female. Studies conducted in other countries showed that many
health practitioners subscribed to this misconception as well
(Dworkin, 2005).
Local HIV and AIDS infection statistics may also contribute
to this perception of lesbian immunity. From Singapore’s Ministry
of Health’s statistics on HIV infections or AIDS (Ministry of
Health, 2008), the trend, from 1985 to 2007, showed that infected
females were increasingly outnumbered by infected males. The
number of women who were infected made up approximately 10% of
the number of infected men over the last three years. In the year
2007, 31 women were affected as compared to 392 men. In the year
2006, 32 women were affected as compared to 325 men. In 2005, the
figures were 30 and 287 for females and males, respectively.
Between 2005 and 2007, the odds of married heterosexual women
being infected were more than twice that of single women (the
statistics for single women do not distinguish between
heterosexual and lesbian women). These small numbers of female
infections may lead Singaporean lesbian women to perceive
themselves as not at risk, since they are female and
predominantly unmarried (as same-sex marriage is not allowed in
Singapore).
Because of this general perception of invulnerability,
lesbian women who are aware of safer sexual behaviors may
therefore not utilize, even with multiple partners, protective
measures (such as dental dams, which are rectangular sheets of
latex placed over the genital region during oral stimulation of
the genital, or rubber gloves, which are used during finger
penetration of the vagina) during risky sexual activities
5
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
(Fishman & Anderson, 2003; Richardson, 2000). Thus, the
perception of lesbian immunity may actually be a basis for
lesbian women not only being more permissive (having multiple
partners) but also being irresponsibly permissive (having unsafe
sex with multiple partners).
Contrary to this perception of immunity, the wide variety of
sexual activities that lesbian women engage in may expose them to
STIs. In Montcalm and Myer’s (2000) sample of 248 lesbian
participants, the majority practiced receiving digital-vaginal
sex (where a partner’s finger is inserted into one’s vagina)
(84.3%), giving digital-vaginal sex (where one inserts a finger
into a partner’s vagina) (87.1%), receiving oral-vaginal sex
(where a partner orally stimulates one’s vagina) (79.0%) and
giving oral-vaginal sex (where one orally stimulates a partner’s
vagina) (81.5%). Sharing of sex toys such as dildos (33.9%),
receiving digital-anal sex (where a partner’s finger is inserted
into one’s anus) (23.4%), giving digital-anal sex (where one
inserts a finger into a partner’s anus) (24.2%) and fisting
(inserting a hand or all fingers into the vagina or anus) (less
than 10% each for vaginal and anal fisting) were much less
popular. About one-quarter of the women had sex involving genital
contact during their partners’ menstruation. A similar
preponderance of sexual activities was found in Morrow and
Allsworth’s (2000) sample of 504 lesbian women, with
approximately 90% of participants practicing oral-vaginal and
digital-vaginal sex. Although HIV infections and AIDS are much
lower for unmarried females (which would include most lesbian
women), exposure to mucous membranes, vaginal secretions, and
menstrual blood increases the risk of acquiring STIs such as
6
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
herpes and chlamydia (Fishman & Anderson, 2003; Marrazzo, Coffey,
& Bingham, 2005). Some lesbian women acquire female bisexual
partners who have had multiple male sexual partners before, thus
increasing their risk of STIs (Dworkin, 2005) such as
trichomoniasis and anogenital warts which can affect women who
never had sex with men (Morrow & Allsworth, 2000). Once infected,
complications are greater in women as compared to men, and women
face a wider variety of problems, such as infertility and chronic
pelvic pain (Diamant, Lever, & Schuster, 2000). Thus, even if the
risk for women is indeed lower, it does not mean there is no risk
at all. As mentioned earlier, sexual activities that involve
infected vaginal fluid and menstrual blood coming into contact
with mucosal membranes, open sores, or cuts increase one’s
susceptibility to STIs. Therefore, lesbian immunity is really an
illusion; lesbian women do not possess immunity to STIs. If
lesbian women are indeed more sexually permissive and
irresponsibly so because of the perception of immunity even
though they really are not immune, this can have important
implications for the sexual health of the lesbian community. The
main objective of this thesis was therefore to examine
perceptions associated with sexual permissiveness, focusing on
acceptability (how acceptable) and risk (how risky) ratings
associated with such permissiveness among lesbian women, and to
contrast these acceptability and risk perceptions against those
of heterosexual women.
In many studies on lesbian women, lesbian women were
recruited on the basis of their self-identification as lesbian
women (e.g., Montcalm & Myer, 2000; Morrow & Allsworth, 2000).
This self-identification approach can be problematic because
7
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
self-identified lesbian women may not include only lesbian women
but also those who are bisexual as well. According to Diamond
(2005b), women with attractions to both genders are more likely
to change their sexual identity over time because, after
acknowledging same-sex attractions, they have the additional step
of weighing how strongly they lean towards each gender. Thus,
some self-identified lesbian women may actually be bisexuals in
the making. Indeed, in Rust’s (1993) retrospective study, 75% of
bisexual respondents once identified themselves as lesbian.
Alternatively, there may be bisexual respondents who identify
themselves as lesbian women because they have a predominant
interest in women even though they are attracted (to different
extents) to both genders (Diamond, 2005b). Many self-identified
lesbian women have also had sex with men (Champion, Wilford,
Shain, & Piper, 2005). Thus, self-identification as a lesbian
women appears to function more as a tool for making sense of
current sexual attractions (Weinberg, Williams, & Proyer, 1994).
Additionally, some women may maintain a lesbian identity for
reasons irrelevant to their sexuality. For instance, one woman
identified as herself as a lesbian simply because of a dislike
for the heterosexual culture (Diamond, 2005b).
To enhance knowledge and understanding, it is meaningful to
make distinctions between bisexual women and lesbian women
(Gangestad, Bailey, & Martin, 2000). Diamond’s (2005a) study on
subgroups of sexual minority women involving “stable lesbians”
(consistently identifying as lesbian), “fluid lesbians” (changing
between sexual identities), and “stable nonlesbians” (never
adopted lesbian sexual identity, bisexual or unlabeled) is a step
forward in this regard. Such a distinction is useful in that it
8
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
was found that “stable lesbians” had the smallest fluctuations in
their emotional and physical attraction to the same sex (Diamond,
2005a). In this light, studies that recruit lesbian women may not
really have only lesbian women in their samples, which would
raise questions regarding the generalizability of their results
to lesbian women. One solution to this problem, which would also
enhance generalizability to lesbian women, is to flesh out the
term “lesbian” in terms of specific criteria, so that potential
participants are not left to their own interpretations of who a
lesbian is. This therefore was the approach adopted in this
thesis. Specifically, lesbian women were defined as women who
experienced emotional and physical attractions towards members of
the same sex only. Such an approach reduced the lesbian sample to
only a subgroup of WSW (women who have sex with women) and helped
to sift out bisexual women as well as those who identified
themselves as lesbian for reasons irrelevant to their sexuality.
In specifying the criteria associated with the term “lesbian”
during recruitment, potential participants were therefore
provided with objective markers to determine their eligibility
for participation.
In many studies on lesbian women, heterosexual women were
not included as a comparison group. While this may be seen as an
acknowledgement of or statement of intent that lesbian women are
a group worthy of study in and of themselves, there may be
inadvertent knowledge gaps that are created regarding what
actually differentiates heterosexual women and lesbian women.
From a pragmatic point of view, when comments are made about
lesbian women’s sexuality (such as those by religious groups),
such comments typically pitch the perceived deviant (lesbian
9
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
women) against the socially acceptable (heterosexual women).
Although this may be perceived as biased or prejudiced,
heterosexual women do offer a familiar reference point for
understanding lesbian women. By asking the same questions of both
groups, it is then possible to accurately quantify both
differences and similarities between the two groups, and in so
doing actually provide for a better understanding of lesbian
women. This thesis therefore examined perceptions associated with
sexual permissiveness in local lesbian women, with local
heterosexual women as a comparison group.
Sexual permissiveness is often conceptualized in terms of
actual behavior (the number of partners one has). However, such
an approach can be severely limiting in recruiting participants,
especially in an Asian context where people tend to be more
conservative, such that fewer people may actually have multiple
partners or are sufficiently bold to be forthcoming about the
number of partners they have. To circumvent this, this thesis
assessed sexual permissiveness from an attitudinal (how
acceptable) perspective and a perceptual (how risky) perspective.
Cross-cultural studies have been done examining sexual attitudes
of French and American participants, where the French were found
to be more permissive (LeGall, Mullet, & Riviere-Shafighi, 2002).
There were also gender comparative studies that found women to be
less sexually permissive (Hendrick & Hendrick, 1987). Risk
perceptions, as examined in this study, pertain to the level of
perceived risk associated with sexual permissiveness. It should
be noted that perceptions of risk need not correspond with actual
risk levels. It is perceived risk, however, that typically
influences behavior rather than actual risk levels. As noted
10
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
previously, lesbian women who were conscious of sexual risks were
more likely to utilize safe sex methods. Therefore, risk
perceptions, not actual levels of risk, were of interest in this
thesis.
Overview of the Current Research
This thesis covered 2 studies. The first study examined
terms relevant to sexual permissiveness. Examining sexual
permissiveness encompassed understanding perceptions of
relationships, sex, and virginity. Focus group discussions with
heterosexual and lesbian women were employed towards this end.
The second study sought to tease out the nuances in acceptability
and risk perceptions when different sexual orientations
(specifically heterosexual or lesbian), genders and sex types
(such as penile-vaginal intercourse or kissing) come into play.
This attempt took the form of a survey questionnaire. The terms
in the questionnaire were clarified using the results of study 1,
so as to maximize similar interpretation of questionnaire items.
Study 1
Before examining differences between lesbian and
heterosexual women in how they perceived acceptability and risk
in sexual permissiveness, it is important first of all to
determine whether heterosexual and lesbian women perceive
relationships, sex, and virginity similarly. This includes
definitions and purposes, such as what it means to be and the
purposes of being in a relationship, having sex, as well as the
importance of virginity (virginity was included because previous
studies had linked it to sexual permissiveness (Pitts & Rahman,
2001). Such a first step is necessary to ensure that when
comparisons are subsequently made concerning perceptions, there
11
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
can be confidence that these perceptions apply to concepts and
terms that are either interpreted similarly by both groups of
women or are known to have different interpretations for the two
groups of women.
A key question for the two groups of women is the question
of what constitutes sex. Answers here have the potential of
revealing common sexual activities that both groups of women are
familiar with. In doing so, the accompanying perceptions of
acceptability and risk of these common sexual activities can then
be assessed with confidence. Eliciting responses on the purposes
of being in a relationship and having sex might also shed light
on differences between lesbian and heterosexual women that could
contribute to the negative stereotype of promiscuity. Knowing the
purposes can also highlight the boundaries and expectations of
being in a relationship for these two groups of women.
Available literature suggests that there are possible
differences between heterosexual and lesbian women in how they
view relationships, sex, and virginity. Heterosexual and lesbian
women, for instance, are likely to differ in their
characteristics of what a romantic relationship is. There is
evidence that heterosexuals equate monogamy with commitment and
non-monogamy with infidelity (Smart, 2006). On the other hand,
those with same-sex attractions recognize sexual non-exclusivity,
which is common among gay-men coupling (Smart, 2006). Therefore,
lesbian women are likely to possess a broader definition of
“relationship”, recognizing non-monogamous relationships as
valid, as compared to their heterosexual counterparts. Given such
a discrepancy, lesbian women may consider other forms of
relationships when responding to the term “relationship”. This is
12
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
problematic because infidelity is a violation of boundary
agreements set between relationship partners. For heterosexuals,
these markers are likely to be fixed (non-monogamy equates
infidelity). For lesbian women, these markers are likely to be
negotiable (non-monogamy may not be a sign of infidelity,
depending on prior agreements). Promiscuity, especially that
leading to truncated relationships, carries connotation of
violation of boundaries. If such a difference in how
relationships are viewed surfaced among local lesbian and
heterosexual women, there would be a need to take this difference
into account before any meaningful comparison could be made in
terms of how acceptability of and risks associated with sexual
permissiveness are perceived.
Secondly, heterosexual and lesbian women may define sex and
virginity differently according to different predominant sexual
activities, different gender of partner, and presence of orgasm.
This has implications for use of the term “sex” for each group of
women. Lesbian women have been reported to engage in oral-vaginal
sex, oral-anal sex, digital-vaginal sex, digital-anal sex and
tribadism (rubbing of one’s genitals against partner’s body part)
(Fishman & Anderson, 2003; Morrow & Allsworth, 2000). Penilevaginal intercourse is naturally absent from lesbian women’s
repertoire. For heterosexual women, however, penile-vaginal
intercourse is possibly the gist of “having sex”. In Pitts and
Rahman’s (2001) study, an overwhelming percentage of female
participants rated penile-vaginal intercourse as sex, compared to
kissing (less than 3%) or oral (less than 35%) or manual (less
than 20%) genital stimulation. Trotter and Alderson (2007) found
similar results, with their female participants most in agreement
13
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
that penile-vaginal intercourse is sex (more than 87%), compared
to deep kissing (less than 4%) or oral (less than 49%) or manual
(less than 28%) simulation.
Some studies suggest that definitions of sex and virginity
loss are defined according to one’s sexual experiences (Bersamin,
Fisher, Walker, Hill, & Grube, 2007). For example, female and
male heterosexuals were likely to define sex as penile-vaginal
intercourse (Trotter & Alderson, 2007). However, other studies
found no relationship between sexual experience and definitions
of sex. Bersamin et al. (2007), for instance, found among their
predominantly heterosexual participants that it was other factors
such as gender of partner, length of dating, and presence of
orgasm that affected definitions of sex. Specifically, their
participants (a) defined a sexual partner as being of an
opposite-sex partner, (b) were also more likely to consider a
larger number of activities as sex if they were in a dating
relationship with the partner for three months, compared to a
partner on a single date, (c) were more likely to consider as sex
oral sex with orgasm than oral sex without orgasm. Interestingly,
however, orgasm was not a criterion for sex when it came to
penile-vaginal or penile-anal intercourse. Participants also
appeared to have different yardsticks for judgment for oppositesex and same-sex partners. For example, they were more likely to
consider oral and manual genital stimulation as sex if the
partner was of the same-sex. Thus, although sexual experience did
not relate to definitions of sex here, it appeared that gender of
partner was nonetheless an important consideration, as is the
type of sexual activity being considered. It should be noted that
for these two studies, the number of lesbian participants were
14
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
not sufficiently large (less than 3% in Pitts & Rahman’s (2001)
sample and less than 2% in Trotter & Alderson’s (2007) sample)
to allow for firm conclusions, so that it remains unclear if
lesbian and heterosexual women do differ in how they define sex
and virginity.
The relationship between sex and virginity is also ambiguous
(Bersamin et al., 2007). Chambers (2007) showed from her sample
of female and male heterosexual college students from the
University of Georgia that perceptions towards what constitutes
sex and virginity loss differed across the participants. For
example, 41.7% of self-identified virgins had engaged oral sex
and 39.1% of those who engaged in oral sex but not sexual
intercourse called themselves non-virgins. This meant that
participants who had oral sex only may or may not classify
themselves as virgins, indicating disparate definitions of
virginity. Participants often described virginity loss in the
context of penile-vaginal intercourse, although some also
consider other types of sex as long as genitals were involved
(Carpenter, 2001; Trotter & Alderson, 2007). Pitts and Rahman
(2001) found that female participants, as compared to male
participants, had a narrower definition of sex and made mention
of the notion of “technical virginity” (such that one engaging in
sexual activities other than penile-vaginal intercourse remains a
virgin). There is likely to be more pressure on young unmarried
women to retain their virginity. Non-virgin status might be
construed as indicating sexual promiscuity.
In sum, study 1 was done to examine and compare how lesbian
and heterosexual women viewed relationships, sex, and virginity.
Method
15
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
Participants
The participants consisted of 38 heterosexual and 38 lesbian
women. Participants were all English-speaking. There were no prerequisites set for participation regarding experience in
relationships and sex because views and opinions were the focus
of this study. Heterosexual women were acquired from the Research
Participants pool of the Department of Psychology at the National
University of Singapore.
Their ages ranged from 19 to 31 years (M = 21.40, SD =
2.30).
Lesbian women were recruited via posters, from local
online portals such as Sayoni.com, and online mailing lists such
as Signel yahoogroups. Their ages ranged from 21 to 34 years of
age (M = 25.40, SD = 3.70).
Lesbian participants were older than heterosexual
participants, t(74) = 5.67, p < .01. Participants were
predominantly Chinese, in the midst of acquiring or already
possessing a degree, and Christian/Catholic, Buddhist, or without
religion. Table 1 provides a summary of the demographics of the
participants in this study.
16
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
Table 1
Demographics Characteristics of Heterosexual (mean age = 21.40,
SD = 2.30) and Lesbian (mean age = 25.40, SD = 3.70) Participants
in Focus Group Discussions
Heterosexual
n
Lesbian
Percentage
n
Percentage
Race
Chinese
27
71.1
35
92.1
Indian
6
15.8
2
5.2
Malay
0
0
1
2.6
Others
5
13.2
0
0
O levels
0
0
1
2.6
Diploma/
0
0
6
15.8
37
97.4
29
76.3
1
2.6
2
5.2
7
18.4
15
39.5
10
26.3
5
13.2
Hindu
2
5.2
0
0
Islam
3
7.9
1
2.6
Others
1
2.6
2
5.2
Education Level
A levels
Undergrad
Postgraduate
Religion
Christian/
Catholic
Buddhist
17
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
No religion
15
39.5
15
39.5
18
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
Procedure
Focus group discussions were utilized to explore
participants’ opinions. This method has an added advantage over
one-to-one interviews because it allows group interaction and
participants to clarify each other’s opinions by asking
questions, making comments, and exchanging stories. This, in
turn, allows for the gathering of a potentially richer set of
data.
Permission was first sought from the NUS Institutional
Review Board (IRB) to conduct the study. Upon obtaining IRB
approval, recruitment for the focus group discussions was done.
Heterosexual women and lesbian women were recruited to
participate in separate focus group discussions. This was done to
take into account the ambivalence towards homosexuality in
Singapore and to minimize any sense of discomfort that may arise
from the different sexual orientations. Participants signed up
for sessions according to their availability.
The discussion itself was semi-structured. Specific
questions were asked in the same sequence in all sessions (see
Appendix A). However, wherever needed, additional questions were
asked for clarification or elaboration. The present author
facilitated all sessions. To prevent a minority from dominating a
group discussion, each participant was given a chance to speak
for each of the structured questions drawn up for discussion.
These sessions took place in various locations, including
tutorial rooms and quiet coffee places, according to the
convenience of participants. Each focus group discussion lasted
between 1 to 2 hours, with group sizes ranging from 4 to 10.
19
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
Seven sessions were conducted for heterosexual women and 6 were
conducted for lesbian women.
Before each session, participants were briefed on the
purpose of the study, after which they signed the consent form
(indicating voluntary participation) and then provided some
demographic data. Participants were encouraged to speak freely
and permission was obtained from participants for the session to
be audio-recorded. The sensitive nature of the study was
acknowledged and participants were told that their responses were
anonymous and their names would not be used in any report. After
each session, participants were thanked. Heterosexual
participants were given participation points for partial
fulfillment of their course requirements and lesbian participants
were given beverages as tokens of appreciation.
Results
Analytic Approach
The present author tape-recorded the focus group discussions
to retain exact content. Transcription of tapes to written text
was then done. That is, participants’ words were written
retaining its original form, including the use of colloquial
English, commonly known as “Singlish” in Singapore which is
characterized by non-words such as “la” and “lo”). These words
did not alter the meanings of the sentences, which were
decipherable through the English words they accompanied. For
transcription, ellipses (…) were used to indicate pauses.
In defining a meaningful unit for analysis, the smallest
text component is a full sentence and the biggest text component
is a paragraph (Laurenceau, Barrett, & Rovine, 2005; Sanderson,
Keiter, Miles, & Yopyk, 2007). A full sentence is defined as an
20
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
expression of one idea, contained within one or a few phrases. A
paragraph is defined as a number of sentences that are of the
same idea, such as elaboration of the same point in a continuous
flow. Questions were divided into distinct segments of the focus
group discussions (Schilling, 2006), namely, relationship, sex,
and virginity. Responses were thus separated according to
segments. Under each segment, responses were further separated
according to sub-questions, such as definition and purpose.
Categories from responses were then generated for each subquestion.
To generate categories, responses were first recoded to
reduce material and yet retain the crux of the contents. This
included deleting unnecessary words and transforming sentences to
short forms (Schilling, 2006). For example, a heterosexual
participant said this in response to expectations of a
relationship partner: “Someone you can completely be yourself
with, like with your bestest bestest best friend, there will be
some secrets you will not able to share, but you know you can
tell him everything and everything is going to be okay… might
fight or whatever… but at the end he will still be there for
you.” From this paragraph, the quality “best friend” was placed
as one category. Subsequently, responses that included “best
friend” defined as “someone you can share everything with” were
placed together.
Although numerical comparisons are at best tentative in
focus group analysis, such comparisons can point to substantive
issues. As an extreme example, if all of the lesbian focus groups
were to raise a particular point while none of the heterosexual
groups were to raise that same point, such a difference in
21
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
numbers may be indicative of important differences between
lesbian and heterosexual women. The use of numbers and numerical
comparisons was thus adopted, although caution was exercised not
to over-interpret the numerical differences.
Tapes were reviewed twice to check for consistency and to
minimize errors. A second heterosexual male Psychology student
(not from the Research Participant pool) also coded one
heterosexual and one lesbian group discussion to ensure the
reliability of coding. Inter-rater agreement was computed here as
total number of agreements minus total number of disagreements,
divided by total number of codes. The inter-rater agreement was
91.4%.
All names used in the following are pseudonyms to protect
participants’ confidentiality.
Defining Relationship
For all groups, participants were asked: “What is a
relationship?”
Participants in both lesbian and heterosexual groups defined
a relationship as romantic involvement with a partner. There
were, however, some differences between groups when discussing
relationship characteristics. Monogamy and commitment were
generated from all discussion groups, but 3 out of the 6 lesbian
groups generated “not necessary monogamous” as a characteristic
of a relationship whereas none of the heterosexual groups did.
The heterosexual and lesbian women groups also had slightly
different ideas about commitment. Heterosexual participants
explained commitment as monogamy. For example, Pat (a
heterosexual participant) stated,
“For me, being in a
relationship means… like ultimately commitment… you know… you
22
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
cannot be allowed to go on a date with some other girl… you know…
or be too close with some other girl… and for me as well.”
Lesbian participants, on the other hand, made a distinction
between commitment and monogamy. Commitment was defined as
working problems out together and making time for each other.
Nic, a lesbian participant, illustrated this as follows,
“Commitment is like… like… I know this is our relationship, we
are going to try it out… even if we have problems, we are going
to work it out between ourselves.”
The lesbian women also recognized open relationships (in
which partners are allowed to be emotionally and/or sexually
involved with more persons outside of the relationship) as valid
forms of romantic relationships but did not endorse such
relationships. Sharon (a lesbian participant) shared as follows,
“Like for me, as long as you and her are on the same page it’s
fine… it depends on the kind of partner you are bringing home…
how many times you have sex with that person… whether that person
is healthy, clean. Basically you have to lay all the cards on the
table… not just having random sex, orgies… I am more into… well,
I believe in open relationships… but at the same time, I am more
into monogamy. That’s my personality.”
Purpose of Being in a Relationship
The question, “What is/are the purpose(s) of being in a
relationship?” was asked. Four reasons were generated here:
emotional intimacy (“best friend”), physical intimacy (“making
love”), marriage, and companionship/support. All groups generated
“emotional intimacy”, which they defined as “being able to share
everything”, as a reason for being in a relationship and
frequently used the term “best friend” to describe this capacity
23
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
to share freely with one’s partner. Nur, a lesbian participant,
shared, “Because my soul mate is my very best friend… I am very
sure of that. To me it’s like… someone who knows you the most…
someone who will be there for your whole life. We know each other
very very well.” Indeed, “best friend” was cited as a prerequisite quality for entering a relationship for 5 out of 7 of
the heterosexual and 4 out of 6 of the lesbian groups.
“Physical intimacy”, which participants defined as sexual
activity with emotional attachment involved (“making love”), was
commonly raised as a reason for being in a relationship. This
purpose of physical intimacy appeared more salient for the
lesbian participants. Four of the 6 lesbian groups, compared to 3
out of 7 heterosexual groups, generated physical intimacy as a
reason for being in a relationship. Heterosexual participants
expressed the notion that women were not as sexual as men. One
heterosexual participant, Kathleen, conveyed, “I don’t think that
a woman… sexually hungry as a man… I don’t think we have that
kind of drive…”
On the other hand, the heterosexual groups were more likely
to cite marriage as a reason for entering a relationship, with 3
out of 7 groups raising this purpose compared to only 1 lesbian
group. Even here, it appears that lesbian women may differ in how
they define marriage. Ami, a lesbian participant, expressed, “You
don’t have to be married to be married. Canada is probably
different but in Singapore you wouldn’t be able to do that right…
you can probably have your own ceremony and it will be as
meaningful…” Marriage in this context appears defined as going
through some kind of traditional or customary marriage ceremony,
24
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
where parents receive cups of tea as respect, rather than
obtaining some legal document.
There were suggested commonalities in the purpose for being
in a relationship. Five of the 7 heterosexual groups and 4 out of
the 6 lesbian groups raised “companionship/support” as a reason.
A heterosexual participant, Lisa, communicated “I guess being in
a relationship means you are sharing your life with somebody…
it’s like what’s happening in my life, then I share with you,
then he knows what’s happening in my life… be like the supportive
role for you.” A lesbian participant also spoke of support as
follows, “someone to support me through my bad times and someone
to share my happiness la… literally la. I need that person to be
involved in my life… and I would like to be involved in that
person’s life.”
Defining Sex
The question “What is sex?” was asked and participants were
likely to cite examples that were relevant to them. For example,
all 7 heterosexual groups brought up penile-vaginal intercourse
as sex compared to just 4 out of the 6 lesbian groups. On the
other hand, more lesbian groups than heterosexual groups brought
up oral sex (all of the lesbian groups vs. none of the
heterosexual groups), digital sex (all lesbian groups vs. 4 out
of 7 heterosexual groups) and external genital stimulation (4 out
of the 6 lesbian groups and none of the heterosexual groups).
Only one heterosexual group and one lesbian group cited anal sex.
All heterosexual groups and 5 out of the 6 lesbian groups
mentioned that orgasm was not a necessary criterion for sex.
However, 3 out of the 6 lesbian groups generated “yes, orgasm is
a criteria for sex”, compared to none of the heterosexual groups.
25
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
These seemingly inconsistent numbers for the lesbian groups could
reflect differing opinions among lesbian women.
Purpose of Having Sex
The question “What is/are the purpose(s) of having sex?” and
participants differentiated between having sex and making love
when giving their reasons for having sex or making love. Having
sex was described as sexual activity without emotional
involvement. Making love was described as sexual activity with
emotional involvement (love). For having sex, all participants
cited physical gratification as the main purpose and physical
attraction as the pre-requisite quality. Five out of the 7
heterosexual groups generated “thrill” or “fun” for purposes of
having sex, while 5 out of the 6 lesbian groups did so. For
making love, participants indicated that they would only sleep
with their relationship partners, indicating that the qualities
for a partner with whom they make love are the same as the
qualities they seek in a relationship partner. All participants
groups mentioned expression of love as a purpose here.
Lesbian groups were more likely to cite “satisfy partner” as
a purpose of lovemaking as compared to heterosexual groups (5 out
of 6 lesbian groups and 3 out of 7 heterosexual groups). In their
elaboration, they brought up the “prevalence of one-way sex”
among lesbians, where one party receives sexual pleasure and the
other party gives sexual pleasure without reciprocation. Jocie (a
lesbian participant) spoke as such, “Especially when your partner
wants it and you are having your menstruation, then you give your
partner la and vice verse. Or when I am very tired from work...
but she wants it… then do lo. After that sleep.” Ying, another
lesbian participant, said, “Low mood for the person who’s
26
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
performing the one-way. Low mood means you don’t feel like having
sex but you want to satisfy your partner.”
There appeared to be greater differences in purpose for
having sex for the reasons of “bring relationship to next level”
and “to procreate”. Three of the 7 heterosexual participant
groups brought up “bring relationship to next level” but none of
lesbian groups did so. This may be attributed to more prolonged
dating periods and later marriages nowadays as compared to older
times, such that heterosexual couples use sex (traditionally
reserved for marriage) as a marker of greater intimacy
(Carpenter, 1998). For lesbian couples, this boundary between the
dating period and a more committed period such as marriage is
likely to be less clear, as marriage was not an option until
recent days. As for “to procreate”, 3 out of the 7 heterosexual
groups brought this up but none of the lesbian groups did so.
This is most likely because lesbian women cannot procreate with
their partners.
Three out of the 6 lesbian groups indicated that they would
need to know if their sex partner was “clean” (i.e., free from
STIs) when participating in sex without emotional involvement. In
contrast, only 2 out of the 7 heterosexual groups brought this
up. This could be a result of the recent promotion of AIDS
awareness program by AFA’s WSW branch (Action for Aids, Women who
have sex with women).
Defining Virginity
Participants were asked: “What is virginity?” and “Is
virginity important?” Participants defined virginity loss in a
few ways: presence of a partner as pre-requisite, tearing of the
hymen, and sexual experience. All groups generated these
27
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
definitions. Definitions that were very stringent included sexual
experience of any nature, such as masturbation and sexual
fantasies.
It was unanimously agreed by all participants that penilevaginal intercourse would render someone a non-virgin. Some
lesbian participants claimed that they did not endorse the
medical definition for virginity (which is having one’s hymen
intact). Heterosexual participants mentioned the importance of
mutual genital involvement, including penile-vaginal intercourse,
for transition to non-virgin status.
Participants perceived virginity as “important”,
“unimportant”, or “not relevant at all”. Some heterosexual
participants (found in 4 out of the 7 heterosexual groups) found
virginity (on the part of both parties) to be important and
sacred, and expressed that sex should be reserved for marriage.
One heterosexual participant, Feng, shared, “If your boyfriend is
a virgin, I think it’s respect… This is the person who you want
to spend the rest of your life with… Giving your virginity is a
good gift to your partner.” Other heterosexual participants
(found in 5 out of the 7 heterosexual groups) reported virginity
to be unimportant, indicating differing views among the
heterosexual participants. On the other hand, only 2 out of the 6
lesbian groups found virginity important and none of the lesbian
participants expressed that sex should be reserved for marriage.
There appeared to be a differentiation between virginity
loss and sexual permissiveness. Sex within the context of a
relationship was considered acceptable among participants. Aisha
(a heterosexual participant) explained: “There is a difference
between being a virgin and being promiscuous… I… I think…
28
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
basically, people tend to think that if you lost your virginity,
you are promiscuous… you had a lot of sex with a lot of people. I
think it’s important (to differentiate) between the two… you
probably don’t want a very loose partner… but I don’t see why you
should have a problem with having a non-virgin… probably she
really did love him.” For lesbian women who suggested that
virginity is important, their argument was linked to sexual
permissiveness as well, not virginity per se. This is similar to
that made by heterosexual women who perceived virginity to be
unimportant. Irene, a lesbian participant, stated, “I think it’s
(virginity) important… in the sense as… how was their attitude
towards it… when they you know… if it’s like anything anything
one… then I will be wa lao (a colloquial term to indicate
disapproval or displeasure). It’s important how they treat it at
first. Not like die die must be virgin, married then you can.
Must still treat it as something sacred… for the right person.”
All lesbian groups suggested that the concept of virginity
is borne out of a patriarchal society. A lesbian participant,
Jer, said, “I think for me… like ya… Over-rated? Because I think
virginity is a social construct. It’s created by a patriarchal
system to perpetrate certain norms. I mean there is no
definition… to me… I don’t really have a definition.”
Discussion
On the whole, heterosexual and lesbian participants had
similar as well as dissimilar views on relationships, sex, and
virginity. Even within similar views, however, there were nuances
within the groups suggesting that terminologies may need to be
better specified. For instance, while both groups desired
commitment and monogamy in their relationships, they explained
29
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
these terms differently. Heterosexual participants saw commitment
as monogamy, but lesbian participants saw commitment and monogamy
as distinct. Lesbian participants were cognizant of nonmonogamous relationships as valid, presumably as long as the two
parties agreed on it. This, in turn, has implications for the
term “relationship”. Specifically, the term that provides a
common platform for both groups is “monogamous relationship”.
Similarities between heterosexual and lesbian participants
can be seen in their reasons for entering into a romantic
relationship. Both groups cited emotional and physical intimacy
as reasons. Intimacy has been found to be a goal of dating and
relationships (Gable, Reis, Impett, & Asher, 2004; McNulty &
Karney, 2004) and is often an experienced result of an
interpersonal process, which includes self-disclosure and
partner-responsiveness. Other researchers have found
communication and partner’s responsiveness when sharing both good
and bad news to be indicative of relationship longevity (2000).
Similarly, in Mackey, Diemer, and O’Brien’s study (Dolan, 2005),
lesbian women described their relationships as “psychologically
intimate”, which they explained as having “a best friend”
(Mackey, Diemer & O’Brien, 2000, p. 215). This suggests that
qualities that facilitate self-disclosure and minimize the
negative effects of conflicts are desired in relationship
partners, whether such partners are of the same or different sex.
Interestingly, more lesbian groups mentioned physical intimacy as
a reason for entering a romantic relationship. Here, however,
heterosexual participants could have been more restrained at
expressing sexual desires, as participants stated that women are
less sexual than men.
30
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
In terms of how relationships are viewed, the most salient
group difference was marriage. The small number of lesbian groups
that brought up marriage could be due to factors such as parental
disapproval, current lack of legal recognition for same-sex
pairings, and the use of different ways to indicate a deeper
level of commitment (e.g., purchase of matching “couple rings”).
The political and social climate of Singapore may also have an
impact on the lesbian women’s views of marriage. Absent from the
penal code of Singapore, lesbian women do not face legal
persecution for same-sex sexual activities. However, unions
(civil unions or marriages) between same-sex partners are not
recognized and child adoption is not extended to same-sex
partners. Therefore, while lesbian women in Singapore have the
liberty to date and form meaningful relationships without legal
persecution, marriage and the prospects of having children would
less likely be part of their considerations of relationships.
For definitions of sex, participants were likely to cite
examples that were relevant to them. This is consistent with past
findings where heterosexuals defined sex as penile-vaginal
intercourse (Berasmin & Fisher, 2007; Trotter & Alderson, 2007).
Lesbian women were likely to engage in activities such as
cunninlingus (oral sex), digital sex, and tribadism (stimulation
of genitals via rubbing against body parts) (Edward S. Herold &
Mewhinney, 1993; Wallerstein, 1996). It should be noted although
lesbian participants did not participate in penile-vaginal
intercourse, they did cite such intercourse as sex. This points
to lesbian women knowing societal norms and definitions of sex.
This is not surprisingly so, as such norms and definitions are
transmitted through peer gossip, confidences between friends, and
31
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
formal sex education that takes place in the schools.
A similar
rationale can be applied to understand lesbian participants’
definitions of virginity. Although lesbian participants did not
find virginity useful as a concept, they were nevertheless able
to generate similar criteria for what constitutes virginity.
Although there is some ambiguity here, it is interesting to
note that lesbian participants from 3 of the 6 groups indicated
that orgasm is important as a criterion for sex, whereas none of
the heterosexual participant groups did so. For lesbian women who
sometimes participate mainly in external stimulation of the
genitals (such as oral or manual stimulation of genitals) during
sexual activity, orgasm may be an important marker of when
foreplay stops and when the actual sex act (perhaps an equivalent
to penetration) begins. Heterosexual and lesbian participants had
similar purposes for sex, such as physical gratification and
making love. Women have been found to engage in sexual activity
for pleasure (Metz & McCarthy, 2007; Patrick, Sells, Giordano, &
Tollerud, 2007) and for a physical expression of intimacy
(Carpenter, 1998). Lesbian participants, however, differed from
heterosexual participants on the purpose of partner satisfaction.
This is seen in the prevalence of one-way sex being cited among
lesbian women, which added to their purposes of sex.
The primacy of penetration in virginity loss was found. This
is similar to previous findings (Carpenter, 2001; Trotter &
Alderson, 2007). As with Chambers’ (2007) heterosexual
participants, in which those who had oral sex only may or may not
classify themselves as virgins, some heterosexual participants in
this study brought up the need for mutual genital involvement for
virginity loss. Therefore, some participants perceived that some
32
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
forms of sex would not take away one’s virginity. Lesbian
participants, on the other hand, do not require involvement of
both genitals to lose one’s virginity. This is likely because
lesbian women’s sex experiences do not usually contain mutual
genital involvement. Instead, they take turns to give and receive
pleasure, or either receive or give pleasure (one-way sex).
None of the lesbian groups expressed that sex should be
retained for marriage, probably because there is currently no
such thing as same-sex marriages in the local context. For
heterosexual groups, there were mixed opinions on virginity,
depending on whether it reflects sexual permissiveness. This is
consistent with the observed shift, which took place gradually
over the last few decades, from a traditional to a relational
orientation towards sexuality (Carpenter, 1998). People who hold
a traditional orientation to sexuality believe that sex should
only take place within marriage and discourage all forms of
sexual activity that does not lead to procreation (Laumann,
Gagnon, Michael, & Michaels, 1994). People who hold a relational
orientation to sexuality are in favor of sexual activity as long
as it takes place within monogamous, committed, and loving
relationships (Edward S. Herold & Mewhinney, 1993). As a result,
people are increasingly more accepting of pre-martial sex in the
context of a loving relationship (Fishman & Anderson, 2003;
Montcalm & Myer, 2000). Regardless of whether sex should be
reserved for marriage, both heterosexual and lesbian participants
were concerned about sexual permissiveness. Virginity was deemed
important if it served as an indicator of sexual permissiveness
and perceived as unimportant if it was not. This indicates that
sexual permissiveness, not virginity, is of interest.
33
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
The findings of the present study show that even though
there are differences, the views and opinions put forth on
relationships, sex, and virginity by the lesbian and heterosexual
participants are in many ways similar. Indeed, there appears to
be a prevailing thread of sexual permissiveness among the
responses of both groups of participants, taking on the form of a
distinction between monogamy versus commitment in how
relationships are viewed as well as the importance of virginity
and how it is defined in the context of sex. This primacy of
sexual permissiveness, as well as the sexual activities cited by
(and therefore familiar to) both heterosexual and lesbian groups
in the present study, indicate the possibility of a common
framework for comparing lesbian and heterosexual women with
respect to different aspects of sexual permissiveness (such as
how acceptable and how risky such permissiveness is).
Study 2
Study 2 was conducted to examine and compare sexual
permissiveness, in terms of acceptability and perceived risk, of
heterosexual and lesbian women. Drawing on the prevailing
environment (e.g., religious groups warning that lesbian women
are sexually permissive and that significant numbers of lesbian
women exchange sex for money or drugs), the documentation that
lesbian women do participate in sexually permissive activities
(in having multiple partners) (e.g., Morrow & Allsworth, 2000),
as well as the perception of lesbian immunity (Montcalm & Myer,
2000), it was thus expected that lesbian women are likely to be
more accepting of sexual permissiveness (hypothesis 1a) and to
perceive less risk in being sexually permissive (hypothesis 1b)
than heterosexual women.
34
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
Although researchers had examined self-perceived risk for
HIV in lesbian women and the types of sexual activities that
lesbian women engage in, these studies did not identify the
specific perceived risk associated with each sexual activity
(Fishman & Anderson, 2003; Montcalm & Myer, 2000). Neither was
the acceptability of these various sexual activities explored. On
the other hand, participants are likely to distinguish between
different types of sexual activities and which activities would
be regarded as “sex”. In Pitts and Rahman’s (2001) study on what
constitutes sex with 190 UK female and 353 US female
undergraduate students (predominantly heterosexual), students
differentiated between deep kissing (involving the tongue),
receiving and giving manual touch to genitals, receiving and
giving oral-genital contact, and penile-vaginal intercourse. Less
than 3% deemed kissing as sex. For receiving manual touch to own
genitals, 15.8% of UK participants and 12.2% of US women
considered it sex. For giving manual touch to partner’s genitals,
15.3% of UK and 11.6% of US participants considered it sex. For
receiving and giving oral sex, 32.1% to 37.7% of the participants
considered it sex. The majority of participants (more than 98%)
deemed penile-vaginal intercourse as sex. The same hierarchy
appeared in Randell and Byers’ (2003) sample of 164 heterosexual
university participants. 1.9% of the female participants
considered tongue kissing as sex. 11% considered receiving, and
15% considered giving, manual touch to the point of orgasm as
sex. Slightly less than 25% of the female participants regarded
receiving and giving oral contact with genitals as sex. More than
95% of participants regarded penile-vaginal intercourse as sex.
Hence, the hierarchy, from the least considered as sex to the
35
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
most, appeared to be as such: kissing, digital sex, oral sex, and
finally penile-vaginal intercourse. The mode (receiving and
giving) of sexual activities did not appear to make a major
difference. Participants from Trotter and Alderson’s (2007) study
also appeared to have different yardsticks for same-sex
activities, being more likely to consider oral and manual
stimulation as sex for same-sex partners. Thus, any examination
of acceptability and risk perceptions would need to take into
account the type of sexual activity as well as the gender of the
partner involved.
It should be noted that studies on sex-related issues, and
not just those focusing on lesbian women, rarely specify what the
relationship context surrounding the sex-related issues was, such
that it is not clear whether the results apply to, for instance,
sex in the context of a monogamous relationship. Results from
Study 1 indicate that relationship context is important and needs
to be taken into account when examining sex-related differences
between lesbian and heterosexual women (e.g., while both lesbian
and heterosexual women in Study 1 desired commitment and monogamy
in their relationships, lesbian women did not equate monogamy
with commitment the way heterosexual women did). To accurately
examine sexual permissiveness as applied to both groups of women,
it is thus imperative to specify the relationship context. Since
the focus of this thesis was on sexual permissiveness, the
relationship context was therefore restricted to only those
settings that are arguably considered as sexually permissive by
both lesbian and heterosexual women. Such settings of permissive
sexual activities include engaging in various sexual activities
36
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
outside of a love relationship and/or with multiple partners and
having one-night stands.
It can be expected that lesbian women differ from
heterosexual women in how acceptable they see sex with same-sex
partners in sexual situations that can be regarded as permissive
(such as one-night stands). Heterosexual participants are less
exposed to same-sex sexuality and possibly less accepting
therefore of such sexual expressions. On the other hand, lesbian
women who step forward to participate in research (i.e., make it
known that they are lesbian) are typically those who have “come
out” and are more accepting of same-sex sexuality than closeted
lesbian and heterosexual women. They are thus more likely to see
sexual activities with female partners—even those that might be
regarded as permissive—as a natural expression of sexuality.
Thus, it was expected that lesbian participants are likely to be
more accepting of permissive sexual activities involving a samesex (female) partner than heterosexual women (hypothesis 2a).
Trotter and Alderson (2007) also found that their
heterosexual participants were more likely to judge an activity
as sex when the sexual partner is of the opposite sex. Since
sexual permissiveness is generally not sanctioned, permissiveness
of activities that are classified as “sex” should be less
accepted than what is classified as “non-sex”. This also implies
that permissiveness with partners of the opposite sex is likely
to be something more greatly frowned upon among heterosexual
women, as seen in typical labels such as being “cheap” or being a
“slut”. On the other hand, the current trend of “pseudo lesbian
acts” among heterosexual women in mainstream media where nonlesbian women behave intimately with other women (such as kissing
37
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
in music videos) may also increase the acceptability of same-sex
activities among heterosexual women. Thus, it was expected that
both lesbian and heterosexual women see permissive sexual
activities involving a same-sex (female) partner as more
acceptable than those involving an opposite-sex (male) partner
(hypothesis 2b).
In comparing lesbian and heterosexual women, there are
potential control variables to be considered. Negative attitudes
towards lesbians, for instance, may bias participants towards
lower ratings of acceptability and higher ratings of risk for
permissive sexual activities involving partners of the same sex.
A similar, though less likely, argument could be made regarding
negative attitudes towards heterosexual women. Such negative
attitudes may therefore need to be controlled for if the purpose
of the study is to attribute differences in acceptability and
risk perceptions to differences in sexual orientation. In the
same manner, knowledge of STI (e.g., STI rates among men and
women) may need to be controlled for because it may affect
acceptability and risk perceptions of permissive sexual
activities. Those with greater STI knowledge may be less
accepting of and may perceive greater risk then those with lesser
STI knowledge, especially in the present context of permissive
sexual activities.
In terms of risk perceptions, the concept of lesbian
immunity (where lesbian women see themselves as safe from STIs)
and popular notions of men being more sexually permissive than
women are likely to result in lesbian women perceiving lower risk
with same-sex partners, as compared to heterosexual women. Thus,
it was expected that lesbian women, as compared to heterosexual
38
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
women, would perceive permissive sexual activities with same-sex
(female) partners as less risky as compared to engaging in the
same sexual activities with opposite-sex (male) partners
(hypothesis 3a). On the other hand, given the various sex
education campaigns (such as safe sex), it is likely that both
lesbian and heterosexual women are somewhat familiar with how
STIs are passed on to another. Thus, both lesbian and
heterosexual women were expected to perceive risk according to
the type of permissive sexual activity. Specifically, it was
expected that both lesbian and heterosexual women would rate
those sexual activities with higher likelihood of transmission of
bodily fluids (namely, oral sex and penile-vaginal intercourse)
as riskier than those activities with lower likelihood of
transmission of bodily fluids (namely, kissing and digital sex),
with penile-vaginal intercourse rated as the riskiest and kissing
as the least risky.
(hypothesis 3b).
In the present study, the link between acceptability of and
risk associated with the various permissive sexual activities was
also explored, in general and separately by each group. As
mentioned previously, perceived risk can influence the use of
safe sex methods. This, in turn, can mean that the activity with
greater risk (one that carries a higher likelihood of
transmission of bodily fluids) is also seen as less acceptable.
Thus, it was expected that, in general, there would be a negative
correlation between perceived risk of permissive sexual
activities and the acceptability of those permissive activities
(hypothesis 4a). However, given that lesbian participants may
think that they are invulnerable to STIs and therefore tend
towards disregarding risk, the negative correlation between risk
39
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
and acceptability for lesbian women was expected to be lower than
that for heterosexual participants (hypothesis 4b). This lower
correlation between risk and acceptability for lesbian women
compared to heterosexual women was expected to apply across
different sexual activities (oral sex, digital sex, etc.)
(hypothesis 4c).
In summary, the hypotheses for this study were as follows:
1a.
Lesbian participants are more likely than heterosexual
participants to be accepting of sexual permissiveness.
1b.
Lesbian participants are more likely than heterosexual
participants to perceive less risk associated with being
sexually permissive.
2a.
Lesbian participants are more likely than heterosexual
participants to be accepting of permissive sexual activities
involving a same-sex (female) partner.
2b.
Both heterosexual and lesbian participants are likely to see
permissive sexual activities involving a same-sex (female)
partner as more acceptable than those involving an oppositesex (male) partner.
3a.
Lesbian participants are more likely than heterosexual
participants to perceive permissive sexual activities with
same-sex (female) partners as less risky compared to
engaging in the same sexual activities with opposite-sex
(male) partners.
3b.
Both heterosexual and lesbian participants are likely to
perceive oral sex and penile-vaginal intercourse as riskier
than kissing and digital sex with penile-vaginal intercourse
as the riskiest and kissing as the least risky.
40
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
4a.
The correlation between perceived risk and acceptability of
permissive sexual activities is negative, such that as level
of risk perceived increases, acceptability decreases.
4b.
The negative correlation between risk and acceptability for
lesbian women was expected to be lower than that for
heterosexual participants.
4c.
The negative correlation between perceived risk and
acceptability for lesbian women of each sexual activity is
expected to be lower than that for heterosexual
participants.
Method
Participants
One hundred and one heterosexual women (mean age = 20.02, SD
= 1.33) and 100 lesbian women (mean age = 20.64, SD = 1.81),
between ages 19 to 26, were recruited for this study. All
heterosexual women and 5 lesbian women were recruited from the
Research Participants pool of the Department of Psychology at the
National University of Singapore. These participants completed
the study for research participation credit. The remaining 95
lesbian women were recruited via posters and local online portals
such as Sayoni, “an online blog and forum for queer women”.
Participants recruited outside of the Research Participants pool
were given $5 as compensation for their time.
To better distinguish lesbian women as well as heterosexual
women and especially to minimize the likelihood of bisexual women
being recruited, recruitment advertisements specified that
potential participants had to be romantically attracted to only
members of the same sex to be considered lesbian, and
41
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
romantically attracted to only members of the opposite sex to be
considered heterosexual. As a checking procedure for sexual
orientation, checking items were included in the survey, asking
participants who identified themselves as lesbians if they had
experienced physical attraction to men in the last two years or
had sexual relationships with men in the past two years. Similar
checking items were included for those identifying themselves as
heterosexual. These checking items confirmed that there were no
bisexual women in the study. Table 2 provides a summary of the
demographics of participants from this sample.
42
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
Table 2
Demographics Characteristics of Heterosexual (mean age = 20.02,
SD = 1.33) and Lesbian Participants (mean age = 20.64, SD = 1.81)
in Survey Study
Lesbian
Heterosexual
n
Percentage
n
Percentage
Race
Chinese
94
93.1
94
94.0
Indian
2
2.0
4
4.0
Malay
4
4.9
0
0.0
Others
1
1.0
2
2.0
O levels
0
0.0
6
6.0
Diploma/
99
98.0
77
77.0
Undergrad
2
2.0
15
15.0
Others
0
0.0
2
2.0
Catholic
6
5.9
7
7.0
Christian
30
29.7
32
32.0
Buddhist
20
19.8
14
13.2
Muslim
5
5.0
4
4.0
Others
6
5.9
3
3.0
34
33.7
40
40.0
95
94.1
77
77.0
Education Level
A levels
Religion
No religion
Employment Status
Studying
43
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
Working
6
5.9
23
23.0
A set of preliminary analyses were conducted to see if
heterosexual and lesbian participants differed on religion,
ethnicity, educational level, employment status, and age. The
categories of some of these demographics variables had to be
reduced in view of low frequencies in certain categories. Thus,
for religion, categories comprised only Buddhists, Catholics,
Christians, freethinkers, and Hindus. Likewise, race was reduced
to “Chinese” and “non-Chinese”, employment status reduced to
“Working” and “Not working”, and educational level reduced to
“Acquired tertiary education” and “Did not acquire tertiary
education”.
Lesbian participants were more likely to be employed, X2(1)
= 12.06, p < .01; to have acquired tertiary education, X2(1) =
11.31, p < .01; and to be older, t(199) = -2.77, p < .001, as
compared to heterosexual participants. Participants did not
differ on religion,
X2(5) = 2.79, p = .73, and ethnicity, X2(1) =
.07, p = .79. Employment status and age were related, r(201) =
.34, p < .001, such that those who were working were older than
those who were not. Educational status and age were also related,
r(201) = .44, p < .001, such that participants who had acquired
tertiary qualifications were older. Lesbian women (M = 20.64, SD
= 1.81) were older than heterosexual women (M = 20.02, SD =
1.33). Therefore, because they were older, lesbian women were
more likely to have graduated with a degree, whereas heterosexual
participants were mostly undergraduates with the highest
qualification of ‘A’ levels (general pre-requisite for university
entry).
44
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
Measures
A survey was constructed to assess acceptability of sexual
permissiveness, perceived risks associated with sexual
permissiveness, acceptability of and risks perceived for various
permissive sexual activities involving partners of both genders,
as well as negative attitudes towards lesbian and heterosexual
women and knowledge of STIs. (See Appendix E for all survey
questions and instructions.)
Acceptability of sexual permissiveness. To assess how
acceptable being sexually permissive was, the Permissiveness
subscale of the Brief Sexual Attitudes Scale (1983) was used (see
Appendix B for items). In this study, items from this subscale
were clarified in parenthesis to maximize similar interpretations
between the two groups of women, as informed by the results of
Study 1 (see Appendix C for these clarified items under ‘General
Sexual Permissiveness’). Thus, for example, an item that
originally read “I do not need to be in a monogamous relationship
with a person to have sex with that person” was edited to “I do
not need to be in a monogamous relationship with a person (where
there is an agreement between the 2 partners to be sexually
and/or romantically involved with only each other) to have sex
with that person”. There were 10 items in the scale and both
heterosexual and lesbian women rated the items on an 8-point
Likert-type scale (1 = absolutely disagree and 8 = absolutely
agree). A mean score was derived for each participant, such that
the highest attainable score for each variable was 8 and the
lowest attainable score was 1. Higher scores indicated greater
acceptability of sexual permissiveness. The Cronbach’s alphas
45
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
were .85 for heterosexual participants and .90 for lesbian
participants.
Perceived risk associated with sexual permissiveness. To
assess perceived risk, items in the previously described
acceptability of sexual permissiveness scale were reworded to
capture the perceived risk of being sexually permissive. For
example, an item that originally read “I would like to have sex
with many partners” was edited to “having sex with many partners
is risky”. Risk here was explicitly defined as sexual risks,
which are sex-related consequences (e.g., STIs) that could arise
from sexual activity. This risk perception was measured on an 8point Likert-type scale (1 = absolutely disagree and 8 =
absolutely agree). There were again 10 items in the scale and a
mean score was derived for each participant, with higher scores
indicating greater perceived risk associated with sexual
permissiveness. The Cronbach’s alphas were .88 for heterosexual
participants and .92 for lesbian participants. See Appendix C for
these items under ‘General Risk Perception’.
Acceptability and risk perception of different permissive
sexual activities. As mentioned, all sexual activities used for
this study denoted sexually permissive settings (multiple
partners, one-night stands, etc.). Although most sexual
activities apply regardless of gender of partner (e.g., oral sex
can be performed with a same-sex or an opposite-sex partner),
there are instances where a sexual activity is restricted to a
particular gender of partner. In particular, penile-vaginal
intercourse requires an opposite-sex partner while the giving of
digital sex can only be done for a same-sex (female) partner. In
all, there were 5 sexual activities in which a same-sex (female)
46
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
partner was involved (French kissing, receiving oral sex, giving
oral sex, receiving digital sex, and giving digital sex) and 5
sexual activities in which an opposite-sex (male) partner was
involved (French kissing, receiving oral sex, giving oral sex,
receiving digital sex, and penile-vaginal intercourse). The
choice of these sexual activities was informed by the findings of
Study 1. Thus, for instance, anal sex was not included in this
study because of the low frequency in which it was generated
among both the heterosexual and lesbian focus groups.
To ensure familiarity with the terms as well as similar
interpretations of each sexual activity, definitions of the
sexual activities were provided. Thus, French kissing was
specified as kissing that involved the tongue, oral sex was
defined as oral stimulation of genitals, digital sex was defined
as penetration of a vagina with finger(s), and penile-vaginal
intercourse was defined as penetration of a vagina with a penis.
To create the scale for acceptability of the various
permissive sexual activities, each of the 10 items from the
acceptability of sexual permissiveness scale was placed on
separate page with all 5 same-sex and 5 opposite-sex sexual
activities (see Appendix D). As before, clarifications were
provided (from “I do not need to be in a monogamous relationship
with a person to have sex with that person” to “I do not need to
be in a monogamous relationship with a person [where there is an
agreement between the 2 partners to be sexually and/or
romantically involved with only each other] to have sex with that
person”) to maximize similar interpretations between the two
groups of women. To allow for acceptability and risk perception
ratings to be provided by both groups of women, some of whom may
47
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
not find a particular sexual activity relevant, the term “target
woman” was used instead of a personal “I” pronoun. This allowed
heterosexual participants to also rate how they perceived the
acceptability and risk associated with permissive same-sex sexual
activities and lesbian women to also provide their perceptions
with respect to permissive opposite-sex activities. The use of
“target woman” also helped reduce social desirability as
participants were likely to feel less threatened by having to
rate someone else instead of themselves.
Participants were then asked to rate how okay
(acceptability) and how risky (risk perception) each of the 10
sexual activities was for the target woman. Thus, every
permissive sexual activity was rated for acceptability and risk
perception. To minimize order effects and to encourage
participants to rate carefully, four versions of this measure
(randomly ordering the list of permissive sexual activities) were
created and used.
Acceptability was measured on an 8-point Likert-type scale
(1 = absolutely not okay and 8 = absolutely okay). Likewise, risk
perception was measured on an 8-point Likert-type scale (1 =
absolutely safe and 8 = absolutely risky). A mean acceptability
score and a mean risk perception score were then derived for each
participant, with the highest attainable score being 8 and the
lowest attainable score being 1. Higher scores for acceptability
meant greater acceptability of that particular permissive sexual
activity, while higher scores for risk perception meant a greater
risk perceived for that particular permissive sexual activity.
For heterosexual participants, the Cronbach’s alphas for
acceptability ranged from .90 to .95 across the different
48
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
permissive sexual activities, while the alphas for risk
perception ranged from .89 to .98. For lesbian participants, the
Cronbach’s alpha for acceptability ranged from .94 to .96, while
the range for risk perception was from .93 to .97.
Negative attitudes towards lesbian and heterosexual women.
Five items were used to measure negative attitudes towards
lesbian women and a corresponding 5 items were used to measure
negative attitudes towards heterosexual women. All participants
completed these 10 items. These items were developed for this
study and derived from negative stereotypes of lesbian women.
Thus, an example item here was “homosexual women are mentally
ill”. Items measuring negative attitudes towards lesbian women
were mirrored in items measuring negative attitudes towards
heterosexual women, such that there was a corresponding
“heterosexual women are mentally ill” item. Ratings were made on
an 8-point Likert-type scale (1 = absolutely disagree and 8 =
absolutely agree). Two mean scores were obtained for each
participant, one reflecting her attitude towards lesbian women
and the other reflecting her attitude towards heterosexual women.
Higher mean scores indicated more negative attitudes. The
Cronbach’s alphas for negative attitudes towards lesbian women
for heterosexual and lesbian participants were .69 and .44
respectively. The Cronbach’s alpha for lesbian participants was
rather low, possibly reflecting greater diversity of views on
what constitutes negative attitudes towards lesbian women.
Results emerging from the use of this scale for lesbian
participants should therefore be interpreted with caution. The
Cronbach’s alphas for negative attitudes towards heterosexual
women for heterosexual and lesbian participants were both .71.
49
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
See Appendix C for these 10 items under ‘Negative Attitudes
towards Lesbian and Heterosexual Women’.
STI knowledge. There were 10 items in this self-developed
scale, which were derived from official HIV statistics in
Singapore. Example items here were “more men than women get STI”
and “women who are affected by STI are more likely to have had
female sexual partners than male sexual partners”. Ratings were
made on 8-point Likert-type scales (1 = absolutely disagree and 8
= absolutely agree). Because items were based on official
statistics, each participant’s responses were dichotomized into
right and wrong. Thus, for an item which was correct,
participants who rated from 5 to 8 (from slightly agree to
absolutely agree) were given a score of 1, while those who rated
from 1 to 4 (from absolutely disagree to slightly disagree) were
given a score of 0. Thus, the highest attainable score was 10 and
the lowest attainable score was zero. Higher scores indicated
greater STI knowledge. See Appendix C for all items.
Demographics. Participants also provided information about
their age, ethnicity, religion, employment status, and highest
educational qualification attained.
Procedures
Participants were first briefed on the study and then asked
to sign their consent if they were willing to participate. They
were then given an instruction sheet on how to complete the
survey and asked to complete the survey independently. Upon
completion, participants were debriefed and participants from the
Research Participant pool were given their 2 credit points, while
other participants were given $5 as compensation for their time.
50
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
All materials and procedures used in this study were approved by
the NUS IRB.
Results
Table 3 shows the means and standard deviations of all of
the study variables.
51
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
Table 3
Means, Adjusted Means (in parenthesis and where applicable), and
Standard Deviations for All Variables in Survey Study
Heterosexual
Lesbian
M
SD
M
SD
NALes
4.07
0.96
2.88
0.64
NAStra
2.23
0.85
2.40
0.88
STI knowledge
7.15
1.59
8.27
1.46
2.62
1.07
3.49
1.33
6.79
.85
6.22
1.17
General sexual
permissiveness
General risk
perception
Acceptability
Female
Kiss
3.54 (3.80)
1.64
5.22 (4.96)
1.79
rO
2.37 (2.59)
1.21
4.05 (3.84)
1.78
gO
2.35 (2.57)
1.20
3.95 (3.73)
1.79
rD
2.44 (2.64)
1.23
4.11 (3.91)
1.80
gD
2.46 (2.65)
1.22
4.19 (4.01)
1.83
Kiss
3.98 (4.11)
1.65
4.80 (4.67)
1.91
rO
2.53 (2.68)
1.30
3.52 (3.37)
1.86
gO
2.50 (2.64)
1.28
3.28 (3.13)
1.89
rD
2.58 (2.72)
1.31
3.59 (3.44)
1.92
PVI
2.18 (2.32)
1.19
2.96 (2.82)
1.89
Male
Risk perceived
52
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
Female
Kiss
3.96
1.67
3.48
1.73
rO
5.80
1.40
5.11
1.69
gO
6.00
1.30
5.34
1.61
rD
5.51
1.40
4.75
1.61
gD
5.37
1.49
4.53
1.70
Kiss
4.03
1.65
3.86
1.88
rO
6.01
1.32
5.75
1.65
gO
6.24
1.18
6.16
1.47
rD
5.63
1.36
5.32
1.74
PVI
7.33
.65
7.14
.88
Male
Note. Negative attitudes towards lesbian women and negative
attitudes towards heterosexual women were abbreviated to “NALes”
and “NAStra” respectively. Sex types were abbreviated to fit the
table. Gender of partner was indicated as “F” or “M”, mode was
represented with “r” (receiving) or “g” (giving), type of sex was
abbreviated to “kiss” (kissing), “O” (oral sex), “D” (digital
sex) and “PVI” (penile-vaginal intercourse).
Potential Control Variables
Negative attitudes towards lesbian women, negative attitudes
towards heterosexual women, and STI were assessed first to see if
they were correlated with acceptability and risk perceptions,
which would then mean that they should be controlled for in the
main analyses. As a rule of thumb, only correlations .40 and
above were regarded as substantially meaningful and therefore
required control in the main analyses (Durrheim & Tredoux, 2004).
53
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
For the various acceptability ratings, Pearson correlations
involving negative attitudes towards lesbian women ranged from .21 to -.42, those involving negative attitudes towards
heterosexual women ranged from .01 to .07, and those correlations
involving STI knowledge ranged from .02 to .12. Table 4 shows all
correlations for acceptability ratings for heterosexual and
lesbian participants.
54
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
Table 4
Correlations for Acceptability Ratings
NALes
NALes
NAStra
STIkno
Fkiss
FrO
FgO
FrD
FgD
Mkiss
MrO
MgO
MrD
PVI
.26**
.07
-.34**
-.37**
-.38**
-.37**
-.36**
-.15
-.25*
-.24*
-.25*
-.19
.06
-.09
.00
.00
-.02
-.02
-.03
.00
.02
-.01
.07
-.08
-.12
-.12
-.10
-.09
-.08
-.16
-.15
-.14
-.06
.75**
.75**
.77**
.76**
.82**
.58**
.58**
.61**
.46**
.99**
.98**
.97**
.63**
.85**
.84**
.83**
.68**
.97**
.96**
.64**
.85**
.85**
.83**
.69**
1.00**
.64**
.84**
.82**
.85**
.65**
.63**
.83**
.81**
.85**
.65**
.71**
.71**
.73**
.64**
1.00**
.98**
.88**
.97**
.89**
NAStra
.17
STIkno
.15
-.03
Fkiss
-.05
-.07
-.00
FrO
-.04
-.03
-.05
.75**
FgO
-.04
-.04
-.05
.72**
.96**
FrD
-.01
-.05
-.05
.74**
.97**
.93**
FgD
.00
-.05
-.01
.78**
.98**
.95**
.97**
Mkiss
-.04
-.15
.01
.90**
.71**
.70**
.71**
.75**
MrO
-.02
-.12
-.03
.62**
.84**
.85**
.81**
.84**
.75**
MgO
-.02
-.11
-.08
.51**
.73**
.79**
.74**
.73**
.67**
.89**
MrD
.00
-.13
-.06
.60**
.79**
.78**
.82**
.81**
.75**
.95**
.88**
.86**
55
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
PVI
-.09
-.13
-.13
.43**
.66**
.72**
.67**
.64**
.59**
.85**
.88**
.86**
Note. Negative attitudes towards lesbian women and negative attitudes towards straight women were abbreviated to “NALes” and
“NAStra” respectively. Knowledge about sexually transmitted infections was abbreviated to “STIkno”. Sex types were
abbreviated to fit the table. Gender of partner was indicated as “F” or “M”, mode was represented with “r” (receiving) or
“g” (giving), type of sex was abbreviated to “kiss” (kissing), “O” (oral sex), “D” (digital sex) and “PVI” (penile-vaginal
intercourse). The upper diagonal portion represents correlation values of heterosexual participants. The lower diagonal
portion represents correlation values of lesbian participants. Heterosexual and lesbian participants numbered 101 and 100
respectively.
*p < .05. **p < .01.
56
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
For the various risk perception ratings, Pearson
correlations involving negative attitudes towards lesbian women
ranged from .07 to .20, those involving negative attitudes
towards heterosexual women ranged from .02 to .19, and those
involving STI knowledge ranged from .03 to .22. Table 5 shows all
correlations for risk perception ratings for heterosexual and
lesbian participants.
57
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
Table 5
Correlations for Risk Perception Ratings
NALes
NALes
NAStra
STIkno
Fkiss
FrO
FgO
FrD
FgD
Mkiss
MrO
MgO
MrD
PVI
.26**
.07
.17
.22*
.24*
.19
.19
.13
.22*
.18
.19
.15
.06
.25*
.14
.08
.18
.18
.23*
.13
.06
.17
-.09
.03
-.06
-.02
-.05
-.06
-.05
-.02
.01
-.04
.13
.62**
.56**
.67**
.69**
.97**
.60**
.54**
.66**
.18
.91**
.84**
.82**
.56**
.95**
.84**
.77**
.35**
.78**
.77**
.50**
.82**
.90**
.67**
.40**
.96**
.62**
.78**
.70**
.94**
.32**
.64**
.77**
.72**
.94**
.30**
.58**
.52**
.64**
.16
.85**
.80**
.39**
.68**
.47**
NAStra
.17
STIkno
.15
-.03
Fkiss
-.03
.09
-.14
FrO
-.09
.01
-.23*
.56**
FgO
-.08
.01
-.17
.51**
.86**
FrD
-.17
.03
-.19
.59**
.83**
.76**
FgD
-.19
.05
-.23*
.62**
.82**
.73**
.93**
Mkiss
-.02
.17
-.11
.93**
.47**
.41**
.53**
.55**
MrO
-.08
.07
-.15
.48**
.83**
.65**
.70**
.69**
.56**
MgO
.05
.09
-.05
.37**
.58**
.69**
.52**
.52**
.47**
.75**
MrD
-.18
.10
-.15
.49**
.65**
.51**
.81**
.77**
.60**
.84**
.61**
.32**
58
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
PVI
.08
.00
.30**
.04
.26**
.34**
.25**
.16
.17
.44**
.57**
.34**
Note. Negative attitudes towards lesbian women and negative attitudes towards straight women were abbreviated to “NALes”
and “NAStra” respectively. Knowledge about sexually transmitted infections was abbreviated to “STIkno”. Sex types were
abbreviated to fit the table. Gender of partner was indicated as “F” or “M”, mode was represented with “r” (receiving)
or “g” (giving), type of sex was abbreviated to “kiss” (kissing), “O” (oral sex), “D” (digital sex) and “PVI” (penilevaginal intercourse). The upper diagonal portion represents correlation values of heterosexual participants. The lower
diagonal portion represents correlation values of lesbian participants. Heterosexual and lesbian participants numbered
101 and 100 respectively.
*p < .05. **p < .01.
59
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
Thus, only negative attitudes towards lesbian women (with a
correlation above .40) was included as a covariate in the main
analyses for acceptability.
As a point of supplementary interest, the two groups of
women were also compared on these negative attitudes and STI
knowledge variables. Here, heterosexual participants (M = 2.23)
and lesbian participants (M = 2.40) did not differ in their
negative attitudes towards heterosexual women, t(199) = -1.39, p
= .166. However, heterosexual participants (M = 4.07) possessed
more negative attitudes towards lesbian women as compared to
lesbian participants (M = 2.88), t(199) = 10.28, p < .001.
On the other hand, lesbian participants (M = 8.27) received
higher scores for STI knowledge as compared to heterosexual
participants (M = 7.15), t(199) = -5.22, p = .001.
Acceptability of Sexual Permissiveness and Perceived Risk
Associated with Sexual Permissiveness
An alpha level of .05 was used for this and all subsequent
analyses. Comparison of the two groups revealed that heterosexual
participants (M = 2.62) were less sexually permissive than
lesbian participants (M = 3.49), t(199) = -5.05, p < .001, thus
supporting hypothesis 1a. Heterosexual participants (M = 6.79)
also perceived more risk associated with sexual permissiveness as
compared to lesbian participants (M = 6.22), t(199) = 3.95, p <
.001, thus supporting hypothesis 1b.
Acceptability of Permissive Sexual Activities
To circumvent the problem of missing cells in a completelycrossed design involving sexual activity and gender of partner as
two separate factors (since women cannot engage in penile-vaginal
intercourse and a woman cannot give a male partner digital sex),
60
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
the 10 sexual activities were collapsed into a single factor.
This factor was therefore made up of French kissing a female
partner, French kissing a male partner, receiving oral sex from a
female partner, receiving oral sex from a male partner, giving
oral sex to a female partner, giving oral sex to a male partner,
receiving digital sex from a female partner, receiving digital
sex from a male partner, giving digital sex to a female partner,
and penile-vaginal intercourse.
A 2 (sexual orientation) X 10 (sexual activity) repeatedmeasures ANCOVA, with sexual activity as the within-subjects
factor and sexual orientation as the between-subjects factor, was
conducted to assess whether there were differences in
acceptability ratings between heterosexual and lesbian
participants, controlling for negative attitudes towards lesbian
women. The Greenhouse-Geisser adjustment was used in instances
where the sphericity assumption was not met.
Table 3 presents the adjusted and unadjusted group means for
acceptability ratings. Results indicated a main effect of sexual
activity, F(9, 1782) = 9.31, p < .001, ŋ
2
= .05, with Greenhouse-
Geisser adjusted dfs = 3.00, 593.64, p < .001. There was also a
main effect of sexual orientation, F(1, 198) = 13.69, p < .001, ŋ
2
= .07. However, these main effects were qualified by an
interaction between sexual orientation and sexual activity,
F(3.00, 593.64) = 7.00, p < .001, ŋ
2
= .03. Simple effect tests
were conducted to follow up on this interaction.
Differences between sexual orientation groups for each
permissive sexual activity. One set of simple effects testing was
conducted to explore whether heterosexual and lesbian
participants differed in how acceptable they saw each of the 10
61
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
permissive sexual activities. To control for inflated Type I
error, Bonferroni corrections were used to adjust the level of
significance used. Because there were 10 comparisons to be made
here, simple effects testing thus proceeded with an alpha of
.005.
Lesbian participants rated French kissing a female partner
(F(1, 198) = 15.55, p < .001, ŋ
2
= .07), receiving oral sex from
a female partner (F(1, 198) = 22.88, p < .001, ŋ
2
= .10), giving
oral sex to a female partner (F(1, 198) = 19.90, p < .001, ŋ
2
=
.09), receiving digital sex from a female partner (F(1, 198) =
23.19, p < .001, ŋ
2
= .11) and giving digital sex to a female
partner (F(1, 198) = 25.55, p < .001, ŋ
2
= .11) as more
acceptable compared to heterosexual participants. The two groups
did not differ for French kissing a male partner (F(1, 198) =
3.21, p = .08, ŋ
2
= .02), receiving oral sex from a male partner
(F(1, 198) = 6.03, p = .02, ŋ
2
= .03), giving oral sex to a male
partner (F(1, 198) = 3.10, p = .08, ŋ
2
= .02), receiving digital
sex from a male partner (F(1, 198) = 6.40, p = .01, ŋ
2
= .03) and
penile-vaginal intercourse (F(1, 198) = 3.24, p = .07, ŋ
2
= .02).
Therefore, lesbian participants rated all same-sex (female)
sexual activities in a permissive setting as more acceptable as
compared to heterosexual women, but the two groups did not differ
on all opposite-sex (male) sexual activities in a permissive
setting. This supports hypothesis 2a.
Differences among permissive sexual activities within each
sexual orientation group. A second set of simple effects tests
was conducted to explore how acceptability ratings differed among
the 10 permissive sexual activities within each sexual
orientation group.
62
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
Two one-way repeated-measures ANCOVAs (again controlling for
negative attitudes towards lesbian women) were conducted, one for
each sexual orientation group. For the heterosexual group,
results indicated a main effect of sexual activity (F(9, 891) =
6.93, p < .001, ŋ
2
= .07, with Greenhouse-Geisser adjusted dfs =
2.62, 259.19, p < .001. Each sexual activity was then tested
against every other sexual activity, resulting in 45 pairwise
comparisons to be performed. Bonferroni corrections were again
used to adjust for Type 1 error, such that each pairwise
comparison was tested with an alpha of .0011. For heterosexual
participants, French kissing a female partner was rated as more
acceptable than giving oral sex to a female partner (t(1, 99) =
11.81, p < .001) and giving oral sex to a male partner (t(1, 99)
= 12.28, p < .001), while penile-vaginal intercourse was rated as
less acceptable than kissing a female partner (t(1, 99) = 17.40,
p < .001) and kissing a male partner (t(1, 99) = 11.69, p <
.001). All other pairwise comparisons did not yield significance.
For lesbian participants, the one-way ANCOVA indicated a
main effect of sexual activity (F(9, 882) = 2.96, p < .05, ŋ
2
=
.03, with Greenhouse-Geisser adjusted dfs = 3.06, 300.30, p =
.03. The same 45 pairwise comparisons were then done, at an alpha
of .0011 to control for inflated Type I error. Using this
significance level, however, none of the pairwise comparisons
emerged as significant. Thus, across both heterosexual and
lesbian participants, it was not the case that permissive sexual
activities involving a same-sex (female) partner were seen as
more acceptable than those involving an opposite-sex (male)
partner. Hypothesis 2b was not supported.
Risk Perceptions Associated with Permissive Sexual Activities
63
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
A 2 (sexual orientation) X 10 (sexual activity) repeatedmeasures ANOVA, with sexual activity as the within-subjects
factor and sexual orientation as the between-subjects factor, was
conducted to assess whether there were differences between
heterosexual and lesbian participants in risk perceptions
associated with permissive sexual activities. There was a main
effect of sexual activity, (F(9, 1791) = 246.76, p < .001, ŋ
2
=
.55, with Greenhouse-Geisser adjusted dfs = 4.00, 796.32, p <
.001. There was also a main effect of sexual orientation, F(1,
199) = 6.60, p < .05, ŋ
2
= .03. These main effects, however, were
qualified by an interaction between sexual orientation and sexual
activity, F(4.00, 796.32) = 4.461, p < .001, ŋ
2
= .022. Simple
effect tests were thus conducted to follow up on the interaction.
Differences between sexual orientation groups for each
permissive sexual activity. One set of simple effects testing was
conducted to explore whether heterosexual and lesbian
participants differed in how risky they saw each of the 10
permissive sexual activities. To control for inflated Type I
error, Bonferroni corrections were used to adjust the level of
significance used. Because there were 10 comparisons to be made
here, simple effects testing thus proceeded with an alpha of
.005.
Heterosexual participants rated receiving oral sex from a
female partner (t(199) = 3.14, p > .01), giving oral sex to a
female partner (t(199) = 3.04, p < .01), receiving digital sex
from a female partner (t(199) = 3.57, p > .001), and giving
digital sex to a female partner (t(199) = 3.75, p < .001) as
riskier compared to lesbian women. The two groups, however, did
not differ in how risky they perceived French kissing a female
64
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
partner (t(199) = 2.00, p = .05), French kissing a male partner
(t(199) = .65, p = .52), receiving oral sex from a male partner
(t(199) = 1.24, p = .22), giving oral sex to a male partner
(t(199) = .39, p = .70), receiving digital sex from a male
partner (t(199) = 1.42, p = .16), and penile-vaginal intercourse
(t(199) = 1.67, p = .10). Therefore, with the exception of French
kissing a female partner, lesbian participants rated permissive
sexual activities with a same-sex (female) partner as less risky
when compared to heterosexual participants. Thus, hypothesis 3a
was mostly supported, except for French kissing a female partner.
Differences among permissive sexual activities within each
sexual orientation group. A second set of simple effects tests
was conducted to explore how risk perceptions differed among the
10 permissive sexual activities within each sexual orientation
group.
A one-way repeated-measures ANOVA with sexual activity as
the factor was first conducted for heterosexual participants
only. Results indicated a main effect of sexual activity, F(9,
900) = 157.03, p < .001, ŋ
2
= .61, with Greenhouse-Geisser
adjusted dfs = 3.36, 335.95, p < .001. Each sexual activity was
then tested against every other sexual activity, resulting in 45
pairwise comparisons to be performed. Bonferroni corrections were
again used to adjust for Type 1 error, such that each pairwise
comparison was tested with an alpha of .0011. For heterosexual
participants, perceived risk for French kissing a female partner
and French kissing a male partner did not differ from each other,
but they were perceived as less risky than all the other sexual
activities, with French kissing a female partner being perceived
as less risky than giving digital sex to a female partner (t(100)
65
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
= -11.24, p = .001), receiving digital sex from a female partner
(t(100) = -12.24, p = .001), receiving digital sex from a male
partner (t(100) = -13.08, p = .001), receiving oral sex from a
female partner (t(100) = -13.62, p = .001), giving oral sex to a
female partner (t(100) = -14.10, p = .001), receiving oral sex
from a male partner (t(100) = -14.92, p = .001), giving oral sex
to a male partner (t(100) = -15.97, p = .001), and penile-vaginal
intercourse (t(100) = -20.14, p = .001). Similarly, French
kissing a male partner was perceived as less risky than giving
digital sex to a female partner (t(100) = 10.00, p = .001),
receiving digital sex from a female partner (t(100) = -11.00, p =
.001), receiving digital sex from a male partner (t(100) = 12.40, p = .001), receiving oral sex from a female partner
(t(100) = 12.34, p = .001), giving oral sex to a female partner
(t(100) = 12.92, p = .001), receiving oral sex from a male
partner (t(100) = -14.30, p = .001), giving oral sex to a male
partner (t(100) = -15.34, p = .001), and penile-vaginal
intercourse (t(100) = -19.81, p = .001).
With regards to digital sex, giving digital sex to a female
partner differed from the other sexual activities. Giving digital
sex to a female partner was rated as less risky than receiving
digital sex from a male partner (t(100) = -5.01, p = .001),
receiving oral sex from a female partner (t(100) = 4.97, p =
.001), giving oral sex to a female partner (t(100) = 6.25, p =
.001), receiving oral sex from a male partner (t(100) = -6.61, p
= .001), giving oral sex to a male partner (t(100) = -8.38, p =
.001), and penile-vaginal intercourse (t(100) = -13.66, p =
.001). On the other hand, receiving digital sex from a female
partner was perceived as less risky than receiving oral sex from
66
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
a female partner (t(100) = 3.73, p = .001), giving oral sex to a
female partner (t(100) = 5.15, p = .001), receiving oral sex from
a male partner (t(100) = -5.60, p = .001), giving oral sex to a
male partner (t(100) = -7.21, p = .001), and penile-vaginal
intercourse. Receiving digital sex from a male partner was
perceived as less risky than receiving oral sex from a male
partner (t(100) = 4.42, p = .001), giving oral sex to a male
partner (t(100) = 5.93, p = .001), and penile-vaginal intercourse
(t(100) = -13.05, p = .001).
With regards to oral sex, receiving oral sex from a female
partner was perceived as less risky than receiving oral sex from
a male partner (t(100) = -4.53, p = .001), giving oral sex from a
male partner (t(100) = -5.76, p = .001), and penile-vaginal
intercourse (t(100) = -11.59, p = .001). On the other hand,
giving oral sex to a female partner was perceived as less risky
than giving oral sex to a male partner (t(100) = -4.62, p = .001)
and penile-vaginal intercourse (t(100) = -11.35, p = .001).
Receiving oral sex from a male partner was perceived as less
risky than penile-vaginal intercourse (t(100) = -10.87, p =
.001), while giving oral sex to a male partner was perceived as
less risky than penile-vaginal intercourse (t(100) = -10.48, p =
.001).
For heterosexual participants, therefore, French kissing and
digital sex were rated as less risky than oral sex (within each
gender) and penile-vaginal intercourse, with kissing perceived as
the least risky and penile-vaginal intercourse as the riskiest.
A hierarchy of perceived risk associated with the different
permissive sexual activities, from the least risky to the
67
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
riskiest, can be deduced from these results and is depicted in
Figure 1.
68
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
Figure 1. Hierarchy of Sex Type Risk Ratings. Sex types within a
single box did not differ from each other. Sex types were
abbreviated. Gender of partner was indicated as “F” or “M”, mode
was represented with “r” (receiving) or “g” (giving), type of sex
was abbreviated to “kiss” (kissing), “O” (oral sex), “D” (digital
sex) and “PVI” (penile-vaginal intercourse).
FrD
MKiss
MrD
FgD
FrO
FKiss
FgO
Least Risky
MrO
MgO
PVI
Most Risky
Heterosexual
Participants
MrD
FrO
FKiss MKiss
Least Risky
FgD FrD
FgO
Lesbian
Participants
MrO
MgO
PVI
Most Risky
69
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
A similar one-way repeated-measures ANOVA was conducted for
lesbian participants only and results indicated a main effect of
sexual activity, F(9, 891) = 106.75, p < .001, ŋ2 = .52, with
Greenhouse-Geisser adjusted dfs = 4.28, 423.53, p < .001. The same
45 pairwise comparisons were then done, at an alpha of .0011 to
control for inflated Type I error. Results here indicated that for
lesbian participants, French kissing a female partner was perceived
as least risky, less so than French kissing a male partner (t(99) =
-5.56, p = .001), giving digital sex to a female partner (t(99) = 7.03, p = .001), receiving digital sex from a female partner (t(99)
= -8.34, p = .001), receiving oral sex from a female partner (t(99)
= -10.20, p = .001), giving oral sex to a female partner (t(99) = 11.24, p = .001), receiving digital sex from a male partner (t(99) =
-10.55, p = .001), receiving oral sex from a male partner (t(99) = 13.19, p = .001), giving oral sex to a male partner (t(99) = -14.83,
p = .001), and penile-vaginal intercourse (t(99) = -19.27, p =
.001). On the other hand, French kissing a male partner was
perceived as less risky than giving digital sex to a female partner
(t(99) = 3.90, p = .001), receiving digital sex from a female
partner (t(99) = 5.16, p = .001), receiving oral sex from a female
partner (t(99) = 6.79, p = .001), giving oral sex to a female
partner (t(99) = 7.77, p = .001), receiving digital sex from a male
partner (t(99) = -8.94, p = .001), receiving oral sex from a male
partner (t(99) = -11.32, p = .001), giving oral sex to a male
partner (t(99) = -13.06, p = .001), and penile-vaginal intercourse
(t(99) = -16.97, p = .001).
70
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
With regards to digital sex, giving digital sex to a female
partner was perceived as less risky than receiving digital sex from
a female partner (t(99) = 3.51, p = .001), receiving oral sex from a
female partner (t(99) = 5.81, p = .001), giving oral sex to a female
partner (t(99) = 6.74, p = .001), receiving digital sex from a male
partner (t(99) = -6.85, p = .001), receiving oral sex from a male
partner (t(99) = -9.25, p = .001), giving oral sex to a male partner
(t(99) = -10.47, p = .001), and penile-vaginal intercourse (t(99) =
-14.73, p = .001). Receiving digital sex from a female partner was
perceived as less risky than receiving oral sex from a female
partner (t(99) = 3.80, p = .001), giving oral sex to a female
partner (t(99) = 5.35, p = .001), receiving digital sex from a male
partner (t(99) = -5.54, p = .001), receiving oral sex from a male
partner (t(99) = -7.89, p = .001), giving oral sex to a male partner
(t(99) = -9.33, p = .001), and penile-vaginal intercourse (t(99) = 14.65, p = .001). Receiving digital sex from a male partner was
rated as less risky than receiving oral sex from a male partner
(t(99) = 4.47, p = .001), giving oral sex to a male partner (t(99) =
5.86, p = .001), and penile-vaginal intercourse (t(99) = -11.00, p =
.001).
With regards to oral sex, receiving oral sex from a female
partner was perceived as less risky than receiving oral sex from a
male partner (t(99) = -6.42, p = .001), giving oral sex to a male
partner (t(99) = -7.21, p = .001), and penile-vaginal intercourse
(t(99) = -12.07, p = .001). Giving oral sex to a female partner was
rated as less risky than giving oral sex to a male partner (t(99) =
-6.74, p = .001) and penile-vaginal intercourse (t(99) = -11.60, p =
71
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
.001). Receiving oral sex from a male partner was rated as less
risky than giving oral sex to a male partner (t(99) = -3.70, p =
.001) and penile-vaginal intercourse (t(99) = -9.42, p = .001).
Giving oral sex to a male partner was rated as less risky than
penile-vaginal intercourse (t(99) = -8.11, p = .001).
Thus, similar to heterosexual participants, lesbian
participants perceived kissing and digital sex as less risky than
oral sex (within each gender) and penile-vaginal intercourse, with
kissing perceived as the least risky and penile-vaginal intercourse
as the riskiest. A similar hierarchy of perceived risks associated
with the different permissive sexual activities can be derived for
lesbian participants; this hierarchy is depicted in Figure 1.
For both heterosexual and lesbian participants, hypothesis 3b
was therefore supported.
Relationship between perceived risk and acceptability of permissive
sexual activities
Pearson correlations were computed to examine this
relationship at a general level and with respect to each permissive
sexual activity.
Perceived risk associated with and acceptability of sexual
permissiveness. The Pearson correlation between risk and
acceptability here was negative as expected, r(201) = -.42, p < .01.
Thus, as risk perceived increased, acceptability of sexual
permissiveness decreased, supporting hypothesis 4a. About 17.39% of
variance overlapped between risk perceived and acceptability of
sexual permissiveness.
72
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
Correlations were also computed for each sexual orientation
group. For heterosexual participants, the correlation between risk
perceived and acceptability of sexual permissiveness was -.40 (p <
.01), while that for lesbian participants was -.34 (p < .01). Using
Fisher’s r-Z transformation, these two correlations were found not
to differ, Ζ = 0.34, ns. Therefore hypothesis 4b was not supported.
Perceived risk associated with and acceptability of the
different permissive sexual activities. The correlations across the
different permissive sexual activities were all negative (from -.32
to -.53), indicating that as risk perceived increased, acceptability
decreased. For heterosexual participants, correlations ranged from .32 to -.53, while for lesbian participants, correlations ranged
from -.34 to -.48.
Using Fisher’s r-Z transformation, the correlations of
heterosexual and lesbian participants for each permissive sexual
activity were found not to differ. Therefore hypothesis 4c was not
supported.
Discussion
The present study was conducted to examine acceptability of
sexual permissiveness as well as perceived risks associated with
sexual permissiveness for lesbian and heterosexual women. Generally,
both heterosexual and lesbian participants were not accepting of
sexual permissiveness. However, lesbian participants were
comparatively more accepting of sexually permissiveness than
heterosexual participants. They also perceived less risk associated
with sexual permissiveness. For both groups of women, as perceived
risk increases, acceptability of sexual permissiveness decreases,
73
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
whether in general or with respect to specific permissive sexual
activities. There was no difference between the two groups in the
association between acceptability of sexual permissiveness and risk
perception, whether in general or with respect to specific
permissive sexual activities. This suggests that both heterosexual
and lesbian participants take risk into account when forming an
attitude towards sexual permissiveness in general or towards a
particular permissive sexual activity. Thus, contrary to what might
be expected from the concept of lesbian immunity, lesbian
participants in this sample seemed to be in tune with the risks
involved in sexual permissiveness and their acceptability of such
permissiveness varied accordingly.
There were no differences between heterosexual and lesbian
participants in their ratings of the acceptability and risk
perception of permissive sexual activities with male partners.
However, heterosexual participants rated all female same-sex sexual
activities as less acceptable. They also rated most female same-sex
sexual activities as riskier (except French-kissing). Therefore,
differences between the two sexual orientation groups arose for
female same-sex sexual activities only. One possible explanation for
this result is that heterosexual participants might be less familiar
with female same-sex sexual activities and therefore rated these as
less acceptable and riskier.
Differences in the acceptability of the different permissive
sexual activities within the heterosexual group of participants
possibly reflect varying levels of relationship commitment.
Heterosexual participants differentiated between kissing (partners
74
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
of both genders) and penile-vaginal intercourse. They also
differentiated between kissing a female partner and giving oral sex
(to partners of both genders). The relationship commitment required
for oral sex is likely greater than that for kissing. In Herold and
Way’s (Billy & Tanfer, 1993) sample of tertiary female students,
oral sex (with men) correlated with level of dating commitment. The
greater the commitment perceived, the more likely they were to
engage in oral sex with their partners. A study on men revealed that
oral sex was also more likely to occur in serious relationships as
well (Halpern-Felsher, Cornell, Kropp, & Tschann, 2005). Oral sex
was seen as something one can engage in before intercourse
(Chambers, 2007). Some studies showed that it was more acceptable
compared to intercourse among teenagers (C. Hendrick et al., 2006).
Thus, oral sex signifies greater commitment than kissing, but less
than penile-vaginal intercourse. In Trotter and Alderson’s (2007)
heterosexual sample, the percentage of female participants who
included each sexual activity in their definition of “sexual
partner” increased from the least intimate to the most (4% for deep
kissing, 40 to 50% for manual stimulation, 60% for oral stimulation
and 80% for penile-vaginal intercourse). That means that 80% of the
participants would consider someone with whom they engage in penilevaginal intercourse with as a sexual partner, but only 4% of the
participants would consider someone with whom they engage in deep
kissing (French kissing) with as a sexual partner. In the current
study, French kissing was similarly rated as most acceptable and
penile-vaginal intercourse as least acceptable. There was also some
distinction between French kissing (female partners only) and giving
75
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
oral sex. However, the rest of the sexual activities were not rated
differently. This implies that the acceptability of other permissive
sexual activities were similar to that of penile-vaginal
intercourse. Lesbian participants did not make any distinctions for
acceptability of sex types at all, suggesting that lesbian
participants did not judge based on types of sexual activities. This
is probably because those with same-sex attractions recognize sexual
non-exclusivity (Smart, 2006), and other considerations, such as
prior agreements, that can come into play.
The differences in risk perception ratings within each sexual
orientation group suggest that gender of sexual partner, mode of
sexual activities and type of sexual activities make a difference in
risk perceived for sexual permissiveness. However, there were
similarities and differences in the way both groups rated.
There were two similarities in the ratings of both groups of
women. The first similarity was in the general hierarchy of sexual
activities. Kissing was rated as least risky, followed by digital
sex, oral sex and penile-vaginal intercourse. The second similarity
was gender difference. Sexual activities with female partners were
generally perceived as less risky than with male partners.
However, lesbian women had more nuanced distinctions with
gender, differentiating between modes (receiving or giving) as well.
Similar distinction between genders occurred for receiving and
giving oral sex. Here, both groups of women rated giving and
receiving oral sex from either gender differently (with female
partners deemed as less risky). However, lesbian participants
further rated kissing and receiving digital sex from either gender
76
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
differently (with female partners deemed as less risky), but
heterosexual participants did not. Heterosexual participants made no
distinction between modes (receiving and giving) at all. They rated
three pairs of sex types equally: receiving and giving digital sex
to a female partner, receiving and giving oral sex to a female
partner, receiving and giving oral sex to a male partner. Lesbian
participants, on the other hand, distinguished between receiving and
giving digital sex to a female partner (with receiving digital as
riskier), and receiving and giving oral sex to a male partner (with
giving oral sex as riskier). Therefore, heterosexual and lesbian
women might have slightly different criteria for risk.
It appears that heterosexual participants decided on risk
level predominantly by sexual activity and then by gender (for oral
sex only). For example, kissing and digital sex were rated similarly
regardless of gender and mode. Oral sex, although differentiated by
gender, were not differentiated by mode. Lesbian participants appear
to decide risk level by all three variables equally (sexual
activity, gender and mode). Sexual activity was differentiated for
different pairs of mode and gender. For example, receiving from a
male partner digital sex was rated differently from receiving from a
male partner oral sex. There were no overlaps. Gender was
differentiated for different modes of every sexual activity. For
example, receiving oral sex from a female partner was rated
differently from receiving oral sex from a male partner. There were
no overlaps. Finally, with the exception of female oral sex, mode
was differentiated for various pairs of sexual activity and gender.
For example, receiving oral sex from a male partner was rated
77
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
differently from giving oral sex to a male partner. There were no
other overlaps.
Both heterosexual and lesbian participants might have judged
level of risk of type of sexual activities based on exposure to
bodily fluids (such as saliva, semen and vaginal fluids). French
kissing involves exchange of saliva and is least associated with
STI. This is because saliva does not contain large amounts of HIV
(Minnesota AIDS Project, 2008), unless cuts, sores or bleedings gums
are present in the mouth (Action for AIDS Singapore). Therefore,
chances of infection through French kissing are low. Digital sex was
rated as less risky than oral sex likely because it involves bodily
fluid from one party only. Infection through digital sex is possible
if there are cuts on the finger(s) used. Both saliva and semen or
vaginal fluid are exchanged in oral sex, therefore it is rated as
riskier than kissing (only saliva) and digital sex (contact with
vaginal fluid only). Penile-vaginal intercourse involves exchange of
vaginal fluid and semen. Local safe-sex campaigns’ focus on penilevaginal intercourse and promotion on condom use could have
contributed to the perception that one is most susceptible to STI
during penile-vaginal intercourse. Therefore it is rated as the
riskiest.
For lesbian participants, the level of control could have been
important in risk perception as well. For digital sex, for example,
it is probably easier to check for cuts on one’s finger/hand than to
ensure that the finger/hand of one’s sexual partner has no open
cuts. Therefore, it is riskier to receive digital sex than to give
digital sex. Giving oral sex to a male partner might expose one to
78
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
semen, whereas receiving oral sex from a male partner does not.
Since saliva is likely perceived as less dangerous (like in French
kissing), receiving oral sex is rated as less risky than giving oral
sex.
General Discussion
Heterosexual and lesbian participants had many similarities
and differences. From study 1, lesbian participants held similar
views of relationships, such as the desire for commitment and
monogamy, emotional and physical intimacy. The main difference
between heterosexual and lesbian participants centered on marriage,
which is currently not permitted by law for same-sex couples. With
regard to sex, the two groups of participants were more dissimilar.
Like previous studies (Berasmin & Fisher, 2007; Trotter & Alderson,
2007), heterosexual groups were more likely to generate penilevaginal intercourse. Lesbian groups were more likely to generate
some forms of sex (such as digital sex) other than penile-vaginal
intercourse. Thus, the propensity of participants to cite what is
salient or relevant to them applies to lesbian women as well.
Orgasms and partner satisfaction were featured more
prominently among lesbian groups, indicating that lesbian women had
different yardsticks for what constitutes sex. In fact, none of the
heterosexual groups indicated orgasm as important for an activity to
be described as “sex”. However, Randall and Byers (2003) found that
their heterosexual participants were more likely to include a
behavior in their definition of sex if orgasm occurred than if it
did not. Trotter and Alderson (2007) had similar results. Perhaps
Asian heterosexual women have a narrower definition of sex. Study 2
79
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
shows that lesbian women also have different yardsticks for other
aspects of sex (acceptability and risk perception) according to
gender of partner.
From study 1, both heterosexual and lesbian participants were
concerned about sexual permissiveness. However, lesbian participants
scored higher on acceptability of sexual permissiveness in study 2.
On closer examination, the difference between groups in
acceptability appears to be the result of sexual partner’s gender.
Both heterosexual and lesbian participants found sexual
permissiveness of sexual activities with male partners equally
acceptable. However, lesbian participants were a lot more accepting
of sexual permissiveness with female partners than heterosexual
participants.
The higher acceptability of sexual permissiveness coupled with
a preference for monogamy in relationships probably mean that
lesbian women are likely to be more sexually permissive outside of a
relationship as compared to heterosexual women. Out of a
relationship, lesbian women are no longer bound by pre-discussed
relationship boundaries. Therefore, they are at liberty to have
sexual activities with any woman. The lack of distinction for
specific permissive sexual activities suggests that prior agreement
on relationship boundaries is of paramount importance to lesbian
women. Without additional information provided, lesbian participants
were unable to judge acceptability of specific permissive sexual
activities. Results from focus group discussions in study 1 support
this argument. For instance, although lesbian participants preferred
monogamy for themselves, they did not invalidate other forms of
80
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
relationships (such as open relationships) as long as partners do
not violate prior agreements of relationship boundaries. As a
lesbian participant (Sharon) said, it is a matter of laying the
cards on the table. Like lesbian participants, heterosexual
participants also preferred monogamy for themselves. However,
heterosexual participants defined relationships as monogamous. So,
heterosexual participants would not recognize a non-monogamous
arrangement as a relationship. These suggest that heterosexual and
lesbian women have different markers of morality. Lesbian
participants decide morality based on explicit agreements made
between two parties, but heterosexual women do so based on whether
monogamy is violated. This is congruent with Smart’s (2006) findings
where heterosexuals equate non-monogamy with infidelity. During
singlehood, heterosexual women are likely to be less sexually
permissive than lesbian women. This is not surprising given the
negative connotations associated with female sexual permissiveness
in the heterosexual world. Words like “slut” are gendered, carrying
meaning for sexually permissive heterosexual women, but not for
their lesbian counterparts or men. Therefore, lesbian participants
were likely more accepting of sexual permissiveness than
heterosexual participants as judged by differing markers of morality
and a lack of social judgment for lesbian permissiveness.
Lesbian women also scored lower on perceived risk associated
with sexual permissiveness. Upon examining group differences on
different sex types, this appears to be the result of gender of
sexual partner and type of sexual activity. There were no
differences between heterosexual and lesbian participants on the
81
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
risk perceived for permissive sexual activities with male partners.
Lesbian participants, however, found permissive sexual activities
with female partners less risky than heterosexual participants.
Lesbian participants also perceived specific permissive sexual
activities with a woman as less risky than with a man. Lesbian
immunity, which is the perception of lesbian women being safe from
STI, might be at work here (Montcalm & Myer, 2000). The way
heterosexual participants differentiated perceived risk of specific
permissive sex activities suggest that this viewpoint of women being
safe from STI is not restricted to lesbian women only. Except for
French kissing and receiving digital sex, heterosexual participants
rated receiving and giving oral sex to a female partner as less
risky than receiving and giving oral sex to a male partner. Lesbian
participants appeared to be more entrenched in lesbian immunity.
This sense of security is not due to disregard for risk, but arises
out of low levels of perceived risk. The low perceived risk might
mean that lesbian women are unlikely to use safe sex methods.
Although lesbian women are likely to be more sexually
permissive than their heterosexual counterparts, there is no
evidence that they are likely to be unfaithful (a consequence of
sexual permissiveness that can result in relationship misery for a
monogamous arrangement). Their preference for monogamy suggests that
lesbian women are likely to form monogamous relationships. As there
were no differences between sexual orientation groups for all
correlations between acceptability of and risk perceived of specific
permissive sexual activities, and no difference between the two
groups for the correlation between acceptability of sexual
82
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
permissiveness and its perceived associated risk, lesbian women are
likely to assign similar weight to risk as heterosexual women. These
correlations were negative for both heterosexual and lesbian
participants. Therefore, it is unlikely that most local lesbian
women exchange sex for money or drugs, as these activities are
highly risky and require high levels of sexual permissiveness. There
is no evidence that lesbian women would perceive such activities to
be low in risk.
Heterosexual participants distinguished between French kissing
and penile-vaginal intercourse, as well as French kissing (a female
partner) and the giving of oral sex. The level of commitment might
affect how heterosexual participants rate specific permissive sexual
activities. From the focus group discussions in study 1,
heterosexual participants shared that engaging in penile-vaginal
intercourse is bringing the relationship to a higher level. This
suggests that penile-vaginal intercourse requires the most
relationship commitment of all sexual activities, followed by oral
sex and digital sex, with kissing as requiring the least. As
commitment increases, women are likely to move from self-protection
to maintaining the relationship, and are less likely to engage in
safe sex because they perceive their partners as monogamous (Tucker,
Elliott, Wenzel, & Hambarsoomian, 2007). Therefore, with the move
towards greater intimacy, sexual permissiveness (which heterosexual
women perceive as a lack of commitment) might be less acceptable
because it violates their expectations of monogamy and its perceived
accompanying low sexual risk. Thus the hierarchy could be perceived
as one of progressive intimacy and commitment.
83
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
The current research extended previous studies by pointing to
similar trends between acceptability of sexual permissiveness (for
heterosexual women only), perceived risk of sexual permissiveness
and definitions of sex for sexual activities. Specifically, what was
least acceptable to be sexually permissive about (penile-vaginal
intercourse), considered riskiest to be sexually permissive about
(penile-vaginal intercourse), was also what had the highest
agreement among participants that it is sex. The hierarchy (for
sexual permissiveness, risk perception and sex definitions) for
sexual activities is as follows: kissing, digital sex (or manual
stimulation), oral sex and penile-vaginal intercourse. In a
committed and monogamous relationship, these sexual activities would
have been acceptable, seen as non-risky and would have been defined
as part of the sexual repertoire. This is supported by Trotter and
Alderson’s (2007) study, in which partners who had dated three
months or more were considered by more participants as sexual
partners, those who were not dating or had only one date. The trend
was similar across all sexual activities. For example, more
participants considered three-month dating partners as sexual
partners when they deep-kissed (French kissed) than non-dating or
one-date partners who deep-kissed. Therefore, the underlying
mechanism for differences between sexual activities for definitions
of sex appears to be the level of perceived required commitment that
accompanies each sexual activity.
This piece of research is the first comparative research on
acceptability of sexual permissiveness and perceived risk associated
with sexual permissiveness in heterosexual and lesbian women. It
84
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
highlighted factors that are linked to sexual permissiveness, such
as gender of partner and type of sexual activity. It also shows that
lesbian women assign similar weight to risk as heterosexual women,
which is different from saying that lesbian women are sexually
permissive because of inherent sexual permissiveness.
Conclusion and Future Research
The results of these studies have implications for the local
lesbian community. Lesbian women are more sexually permissive than
heterosexual women and at the same time, they perceive less risk.
Such perceptions might contribute towards lesbian women not taking
protective measures during sex. If lesbian women are indeed more
likely to be sexually permissive outside of a relationship, they
will benefit from taking safety measures during sex with their
sexual partners. Since the association between general sexual
permissiveness and general risk perception did not differ between
sexual orientation groups, the campaign for safe sex may be
effective. Although female same-sex sexual activities are likely to
hold less risk than heterosexual sexual activities, there is still
an element of risk. If nothing is done, there may be a higher
infection rate among lesbian women in future. Therefore, the results
point to education regarding safe sex among local lesbian women.
Future research should examine actual instances of sexual
permissiveness and local lesbian HIV rates. It is also worthwhile to
explore the link between sexual permissiveness and relationship
longevity in the lesbian community, as well as what lesbian women
consider as sexually permissive. For instance, experiencing a few
85
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
one-night stands might not be considered sexually permissive in the
lesbian community.
Limitations
This study did not cover the whole spectrum of women who are
sexually attracted to women. The findings of this study are
restricted to those who identify as lesbian and who are physically
and romantically attracted to members of the same sex only.
Respondents in this study were generally well educated and
between ages 19 to 26. Heterosexual participants were acquired from
the research pool of the department of Psychology. Majority of
lesbian participants were in the midst of acquiring or had acquired
tertiary education. Their sexual experience might differ from those
who had not acquired such qualifications. Hence, different findings
might occur with different samples of students or other age groups.
86
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
References
Action for AIDS Singapore. (n.d.). Frequently asked questions
Retrieved December 30, 2008, from
http://www.afa.org.sg/faq.asp
Bersamin, M. M., Fisher, D. A., Walker, S., Hill, D. L., & Grube, J.
W. (2007). Defining virginity and abstinence: Adolescents'
interpretations of sexual behaviors. Journal of Adolescent
Health, 41(2), 182-188.
Billy, J. O. G., & Tanfer, K. (1993). The sexual behavior of men in
the United States. Family Planning Perspectives, 25, 52-60.
Carpenter, L. M. (1998). From girls into women: Scripts for
sexuality and romance in Seventeen magazine, 1974-1994.
Journal of Sex Research, 35(2), 158-168.
Carpenter, L. M. (2001). The ambiguity of "having sex": The
subjective experience of virginity loss in the United States.
Journal of Sex Research, 38(2), 127-139.
Chambers, W. C. (2007). Oral sex: Varied behaviors and perceptions
in a college population. Journal of Sex Research, 44(1), 2842.
Champion, J. D., Wilford, K., Shain, R. N., & Piper, J. M. (2005).
Risk and protective behaviours of bisexual minority women: a
qualitative analysis. International nursing review, 52, 115122.
Diamant, A. L., Lever, J., & Schuster, M. A. (2000). Lesbians'
sexual activities and efforts to reduce risks for sexually
transmitted diseases. Journal of the Gay & Lesbian Medical
Assn, 4(2), 41-48.
87
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
Diamond, L. M. (2005a). A new view of lesbian subtypes: stable
versus fluid identity trajectories over an 8-year period.
Psychology of Women Quarterly, 29, 119-128.
Diamond, L. M. (2005b). What we got wrong about sexual identity
development: unexpected findings from a longitudinal study of
young women. In A. M. Omoto & H. S. Kurtzman (Eds.), Sexual
orientation and mental health (pp. 73- 94). Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association.
Dolan, K. A. (2005). Lesbian women and sexual health: the social
construction of risk and susceptibility. New York: Haworth
Press.
Durrheim, K., & Tredoux, C. (2004). Numbers, hypotheses and
conclusions: a course in statistics for the Social Sciences.
Juta & Company Limited.
Dworkin, S. L. (2005). Who is epidemiologically fathomable in the
HIV?AIDS epidemic? Gender, sexuality, and intersectionality in
public health. Culture, Health & Sexuality, 7(6), 615-623.
Fishman, S. J., & Anderson, E. H. (2003). Perception of HIV and
safer sexual behaviors among lesbians. JANAC: Journal of the
Association of Nurses in AIDS Care, 14(6), 48-55.
Gable, S. L., Reis, H. T., Impett, E. A., & Asher, E. R. (2004).
What do you do when things go right? The intrapersonal and
interpersonal benefits of sharing positive events. Journal of
Personality and Social psychology, 87(2), 228-245.
Gangestad, S. W., Bailey, J. M., & Martin, N. G. (2000). Taxometric
analyses of sexual orientation and gender identity. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 1109-1121.
88
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
Gershman, H. (1997). Sexual permissiveness and its consequences.
Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas Publisher.
Halpern-Felsher, B. L., Cornell, J. L., Kropp, R. Y., & Tschann, J.
M. (2005). Oral versus vaginal sex among adolescents:
perceptions, attitudes, and behavior. Pediatrics, 115, 845851.
Harley, M. (2003). Straight talk: the myths and facts about
homosexuality: Focus on the Family.
Hendrick, C., Hendrick, S. S., & Reich, D. A. (2006). The Brief
Sexual Attitudes Scale. Journal of Sex Research, 43(1), 76-86.
Hendrick, S. S., & Hendrick, C. (1987). Love and sex attitudes and
religious beliefs. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology,
5, 391-398.
Herold, E. S., & Mewhinney, D.-M. K. (1993). Gender differences in
casual sex and AIDS prevention: A survey of dating bars.
Journal of Sex Research, 30(1), 36-42.
Herold, E. S., & Way, L. (1983). Oral-genital sexual behavior in a
sample of university females. Journal of Sex Research, 19,
327-338.
Laumann, E. O., Gagnon, J. H., Michael, R. T., & Michaels, S.
(1994). The social organization of sexuality: sexual practices
in the United States. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Laurenceau, J. P., Barrett, L. F., & Rovine, M. J. (2005). The
interpersonal process model of intimacy in marriage. Journal
of family Psychology, 19(2), 314-323.
89
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
LeGall, A., Mullet, E., & Riviere-Shafighi, S. (2002). Age,
religious beliefs, and sexual attitudes. The Journal of Sex
Research, 39, 207-216.
Macfarquhar, N. (2008, December 18). In a first, gay rights are
pressed at the U.N. The New York Times.
Retrieved January
15, 2009, from
http://www.nytimes.com/2008/12/19/world/19nations.html?ref=wor
ld
Mackey, R. A., Diemer, M. A., & O'Brien, B. A. (2000). Psychological
intimacy in the lasting relationships of heterosexual and
same-gender couples- statistical data included. Sex roles,
43(3-4), 201-227.
Markey, P. M., & Markey, C. N. (2007). The interpersonal meaning of
sexual promiscuity. Journal of Research in Personality, 41,
1199-1212.
Marrazzo, J. M., Coffey, P., & Bingham, A. (2005). Sexual practices,
risk perception and knowledge of sexually transmitted disease
risk among lesbian and bisexual women. Perspectives on Sexual
and Reproductive Health, 37(1), 6-12.
McNulty, J. K., & Karney, B. R. (2004). Positive expectations in the
early years of marriage: should couples expect the best or
brace for the worst? Journal of Personality and Social
psychology, 86(5), 729-743.
Metz, M. E., & McCarthy, B. W. (2007). The "good-enough sex" model
for couple sexual satisfaction. Sexual and relationship
therapy, 22(3), 351-362.
90
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
Ministry of Health, S. (2008). Ministry of Health, Singapore: HIV
stats. Retrieved. from
http://www.moh.gov.sg/mohcorp/statistics.aspx?id=246#table1
Minnesota AIDS Project. HIV Transmission
Retrieved December 30,
2008, from http://www.mnaidsproject.org/learn/transmission.htm
Montcalm, D. M., & Myer, L. L. (2000). Lesbian immunity from
HIV/AIDS: Fact or fiction? Journal of Lesbian Studies, 4(2),
131-147.
Morrow, K., & Allsworth, J. (2000). Sexual risk in lesbians and
bisexual women. Journal of the Gay and Lesbian Medical
Association, 4(4), 159-165.
Patrick, S., Sells, J. N., Giordano, F. G., & Tollerud, T. R.
(2007). Intimacy, differentiation, and personality variables
as predictors of marital satisfaction. The family journal,
15(4), 359-367.
Pitts, M., & Rahman, Q. (2001). Which behaviors constitute "having
sex" among university students in the UK? Archives of Sexual
Behavior, 30(2), 169-176.
Ramesh, S. (2007, September 22). PM Lee fielded questions on gays,
foreign talent at NUS forum.
Retrieved December 24, 2008,
from
http://www.channelnewsasia.com/stories/singaporelocalnews/view
/301420/1/.html
Randall, H. E., & Byers, E. S. (2003). What is sex? Students'
definitions of having sex, sexual partner, and unfaithful
sexual behaviour. The Canadian journal of human sexuality,
12(2), 87-96.
91
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
Richardson, D. (2000). The social construction of immunity: HIV risk
perception and prevention among lesbians and bisexual women.
Culture, Health & Sexuality, 2(1), 33-49.
Rust, P. (1993). Coming out in the age of social constructionism:
sexual identity formation among lesbians and bisexual women.
Gender and Society, 7, 50-77.
Sanderson, C. A., Keiter, E. J., Miles, M. G., & Yopyk, D. J.
(2007). The association between intimacy goals and plans for
initiating dating relationships. Personal Relationships,
14(2), 225-243.
Schilling, J. (2006). On the pragmatics of qualitative assessment,
designing the process for content analysis. European journal
of psychological assessment, 22(1), 28-37.
Smart, C. (2006). Review of the state of affairs: explorations in
infidelity and commitment. Sexualities, 9(2), 259-262.
Trotter, E. C., & Alderson, K. G. (2007). University students'
definitions of having sex, sexual partner, and virginity loss:
the influence of participant gender, sexual experience, and
contextual factors. The Canadian journal of human sexuality,
16(1-2), 11-29.
Tucker, J. S., Elliott, M. N., Wenzel, S. L., & Hambarsoomian, K.
(2007). Relationship commitment and its implications for
unprotected sex among impoverished women living in shelters
and low-income housing in Los Angeles County. Health
Psychology, 26(5), 644-649.
Wallerstein, J. S. (1996). The psychological tasks of marriage: part
2. American journal of orthopsychiatry, 66(2), 217-227.
92
Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception
Weinberg, M. S., Williams, C. J., & Proyer, D. W. (1994). Dual
attraction: understanding bisexuality. New York: Oxford
University Press.
Widom, C. S., & Kuhms, J. B. (1996). Childhood victimization and
subsequent risk for promiscuity, prostitution, teenage
pregnancy: a prospective study. American Journal of Public
Health, 86(11), 1607-1612.
Wong, K. H. (2008, December 7). Surge in HIV cases among gay men The
Straits Times, p 16.
93
[...]... comparing lesbian and heterosexual women with respect to different aspects of sexual permissiveness (such as how acceptable and how risky such permissiveness is) Study 2 Study 2 was conducted to examine and compare sexual permissiveness, in terms of acceptability and perceived risk, of heterosexual and lesbian women Drawing on the prevailing environment (e.g., religious groups warning that lesbian women. .. Study 1 indicate that relationship context is important and needs to be taken into account when examining sex-related differences between lesbian and heterosexual women (e.g., while both lesbian and heterosexual women in Study 1 desired commitment and monogamy in their relationships, lesbian women did not equate monogamy with commitment the way heterosexual women did) To accurately examine sexual permissiveness. .. terms in the questionnaire were clarified using the results of study 1, so as to maximize similar interpretation of questionnaire items Study 1 Before examining differences between lesbian and heterosexual women in how they perceived acceptability and risk in sexual permissiveness, it is important first of all to determine whether heterosexual and lesbian women perceive relationships, sex, and virginity... sexual permissiveness (hypothesis 1a) and to perceive less risk in being sexually permissive (hypothesis 1b) than heterosexual women 34 Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception Although researchers had examined self-perceived risk for HIV in lesbian women and the types of sexual activities that lesbian women engage in, these studies did not identify the specific perceived risk associated with each sexual. .. encompassed understanding perceptions of relationships, sex, and virginity Focus group discussions with heterosexual and lesbian women were employed towards this end The second study sought to tease out the nuances in acceptability and risk perceptions when different sexual orientations (specifically heterosexual or lesbian) , genders and sex types (such as penile-vaginal intercourse or kissing) come into play... activities other than penile-vaginal intercourse remains a virgin) There is likely to be more pressure on young unmarried women to retain their virginity Non-virgin status might be construed as indicating sexual promiscuity In sum, study 1 was done to examine and compare how lesbian and heterosexual women viewed relationships, sex, and virginity Method 15 Sexual Permissiveness & Risk Perception Participants... women are familiar with In doing so, the accompanying perceptions of acceptability and risk of these common sexual activities can then be assessed with confidence Eliciting responses on the purposes of being in a relationship and having sex might also shed light on differences between lesbian and heterosexual women that could contribute to the negative stereotype of promiscuity Knowing the purposes can... boundaries and expectations of being in a relationship for these two groups of women Available literature suggests that there are possible differences between heterosexual and lesbian women in how they view relationships, sex, and virginity Heterosexual and lesbian women, for instance, are likely to differ in their characteristics of what a romantic relationship is There is evidence that heterosexuals... other hand, more lesbian groups than heterosexual groups brought up oral sex (all of the lesbian groups vs none of the heterosexual groups), digital sex (all lesbian groups vs 4 out of 7 heterosexual groups) and external genital stimulation (4 out of the 6 lesbian groups and none of the heterosexual groups) Only one heterosexual group and one lesbian group cited anal sex All heterosexual groups and 5... acceptability of and risks associated with sexual permissiveness are perceived Secondly, heterosexual and lesbian women may define sex and virginity differently according to different predominant sexual activities, different gender of partner, and presence of orgasm This has implications for use of the term “sex” for each group of women Lesbian women have been reported to engage in oral-vaginal sex, oral-anal ... acceptable) and risk (how risky) ratings associated with such permissiveness among lesbian women, and to contrast these acceptability and risk perceptions against those of heterosexual women In many... 2005 and 2007, the odds of married heterosexual women being infected were more than twice that of single women (the statistics for single women not distinguish between heterosexual and lesbian women) ... possible differences between heterosexual and lesbian women in how they view relationships, sex, and virginity Heterosexual and lesbian women, for instance, are likely to differ in their characteristics