Tài liệu hạn chế xem trước, để xem đầy đủ mời bạn chọn Tải xuống
1
/ 70 trang
THÔNG TIN TÀI LIỆU
Thông tin cơ bản
Định dạng
Số trang
70
Dung lượng
261,35 KB
Nội dung
EATING THE HUMBLE PIE:
A NON-DEFENSIVE RESPONSE TO FAILURES
AUDREY NG
NATIONAL UNIVERSITY OF SINGAPORE
2012
EATING THE HUMBLE PIE:
A NON-DEFENSIVE RESPONSE TO FAILURES
AUDREY NG
(B.S.Sc (Hons), NUS)
A THESIS SUBMITTED
FOR THE DEGREE OF MASTER OF SOCIAL SCIENCES
DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY
NATIONAL UNIVERSITY OF SINGAPORE
2012
i
DECLARATION
I hereby declare that the thesis is my original work and it has been written by me in its
entirety.
I have duly acknowledged all the sources of the information which have been sued in the
thesis
This thesis has also not been submitted for any degree in any university previously.
__________________________
Audrey Ng Shuhui
27 February 2013
ii
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This thesis had been a major undertaking in my graduate days and I have the following to
thank for the successful completion of it:
Dr. Eddie Tong, for his guidance;
The ISM students, for their data collection and data entry
And, all my friends.
iii
TABLE OF CONTENT
PAGE
DECLARATION
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
TABLE OF CONTENT
SUMMARY
LIST OF TABLES
i
ii
iii
iv
v
CHAPTER ONE
INTRODUCTION
a. Nature of Humility
b. Humility: A Character Strength
c. Humility: Non-Defensive Response to Failure
d. Current Research and Methodology Considerations
1
1
4
6
9
CHAPTER TWO
EXPERIMENT ONE
a. Overview
b. Method
c. Results
d. Discussion
12
12
15
19
CHAPTER THREE
EXPERIMENT TWO
a. Overview
b. Method
c. Results
d. Discussion
21
23
26
31
CHAPTER FOUR
EXPERIMENT THREE
a. Overview
b. Method
c. Results
d. Discussion
34
35
36
40
CHAPTER FIVE
GENERAL DISCUSSION
a. Reconciling the findings with Literature
b. Further Implications and Conclusions
41
43
45
REFERENCES
52
iv
SUMMARY
Humility has been classified as a character strength and has been thought to bestow positive life
outcomes. This research aimed to examine the effects of humility in response to failures. I
hypothesized that humble individuals cope with failures in a non-defensive manner. Across three
experiments, it has been demonstrated that after receiving negative feedback individuals induced
to feel humble do not show over-activation of positive self-concepts neither do they displayed a
diminished activation of negative self-concepts compared to the non-humbled individuals. That
is, they would not deliberately magnify their merits nor diminish their weaknesses in order to
cope with an ego-threat. How these findings reconcile with the nature of humility is discussed.
Comparisons between humility and low and high self-esteem were also reviewed.
v
LIST OF TABLES
PAGE
TABLE 1
Percentage of Humility Traits in Participants Recalled Descriptions (Experiments
1 to 3)
47
TABLE 2
Proportion of Positive Traits listed across conditions (Experiment 1)
48
TABLE 3
Response Tendencies: Mean Number of self versus nonself selections across
conditions (Experiment 2 & 3)
49
TABLE 4
Means of Reaction Time to Positive and Negative Self-concepts across conditions
(Experiment 2)
50
TABLE 5
Means of Reaction Time to Positive and Negative Self-concepts across conditions
(Experiment 3)
51
1
CHAPTER ONE
INTRODUCTION
Research on human character strengths and virtues include the study of humility.
Humility has been classified as a character strength (Peterson & Seligman, 2004) and has
been thought to bestow positive life outcomes (e.g., Emmons, 1999; Sandage, 1999;
Sandage & Wiens, 2001; Worthington, 1998). Although the benefits of humility have
been proposed by both psychologists and religious leaders alike, the study of humility
and its effects on basic psychological effects still remain limited. In particular, questions
remains regarding how humble people respond to failure that threatens the self (i.e., egothreat) because of the personal shortcoming it communicates. Research has found that the
manner in which a person responds to failure has implications for subsequent adjustment
and well-being. If humility is a character strengths that offers resources needed for
effective coping, does it also enable a constructive response to ego-threats? In this
research, I aim to fill a gap in humility research by examining the effects of humility in
face of failures.
Nature of Humility
The study of humility can be traced back to the teachings of theology and
philosophy, many of which expound the merits of being humble (e.g., Richards, 1992).
Dictionary definitions tend to portray humility negatively and associate it with low selfregard and unworthiness. For example, the Longman Dictionary defines humility as
considering the self as less important than others whereas the Oxford English Dictionary
defines humility as having a low view of one’s importance. Note that although these
definitions are confined to the layperson’s perception, studies have found exceptions. For
2
example, in a study by Exline and Greyer (2004), the researchers found that American
college students perceive humility as a psychological strength and do not equate humility
with low self-esteem.
Still the academias painted a more favorable picture and conceptualized humility
much more accurately. In particular, humble individuals are said to have an assured sense
of self-worth (Tangney, 2002; Exline & Greyer, 2004). Psychologists have posited that
this assured sense of self-worth entails humble individuals knowing their strengths and
limitations. This does not mean that humble people know themselves perfectly well.
Rather, it implies that humble individuals are willing to see themselves accurately, to
know both their strengths and weaknesses, and they would not deliberately magnify their
merits nor diminish their weaknesses (Means, Wilson, Sturm, & Biron, 1990; Peterson &
Seligman, 2004; Exline & Greyer, 2004).
Consistent with their strong sense of self-worth, experts have proposed that
humble individuals are able to acknowledge and accept their shortcomings (Sandage,
1999; Emmons, 1999; Jennings et al., 2005). They are thought to be willing to
acknowledge and accept information of the self even if they are negative, and would not
reject or distort this negative information so as to protect their self-image (Tangney,
2000). As noted by Yancey (2000), one reason why early Christian writers are often
perceived as uniquely humble is their ability to acknowledge their mistakes and
shortcomings. Consistently, Myers (1995, 2000) observed that humble people exhibit
very little self-serving bias and tend not to take extra credit for their accomplishments.
These observations reiterate that humble people are assured individuals who tend not to
3
magnify their strengths or ignore their weaknesses to feel better about themselves (Means
et al., 1990; Peterson & Seligman, 2004; Exline & Greyer, 2004).
One need to note that humility is not the same as self-deprecation in which the
self is disparaged, often for self-serving reasons (e.g., to lower expectations, to seek
social approval). A study by Gibson (2007) demonstrated that individuals who selfdeprecate are not interested in having an accurate sense of self. Humility is also not
modesty. A modest person behaves in a socially approved manner, such as not dressing in
a flashy manner or not taking credit for a success, but may privately have a sense of
superiority (Hineline, 1991; Peterson & Seligman, 2004). A humble person, on the other
hand, behaves in accordance to his/her measured self-views and resists seeing
himself/herself as more important than others.
Humble individuals are also known to be not arrogant for these reasons. They are
thought to be sensitive and responsive to the feelings of other people (Exline, Baumeister,
Bushman, Campbell, & Finkel, 2004; Means et al., 1990; Rowatt et al., 2006; Tangney,
2000). They respect the worth of each individual, and do not put people down (Means et
al., 1990; Roberts, 1983; Worthington, 1998). In fact, studies have found that college
students who are humble in their achievements are preferred by their fellow schoolmates
(Hareli & Weiner, 2000; Sedikides, Gregg, & Hart, 2007). The fact that humble people
do not self-enhance or self-deprecate could also make them more likeable and respected
in the eyes of others.
Further, because humble people are not defensive about their weaknesses and
have the desire to improve themselves, they are often thought to be open to correction
(Tangney, 2002; Templeton, 1998). They are ready to change themselves or make
4
amendments for their mistakes (Davis, Worthington, & Hook, 2010; Exline et al., 2004;
Means et al., 1990; Rowatt et al., 2006). Theorists have also noted a strong sense of
spirituality/religiosity in humble people (Powers et al., 2007; Rowatt et al., 2006;
Tangney, 2002). The strong religious beliefs that humble people are thought to hold
might be a channel by which they learn about themselves, keep their achievements in
perspective, acknowledge and improve upon on their weaknesses, and maintain a
respectful and non-boastful approach in their relationships with others. All in all, humility
can be thought as a dispositional character strength that brings about positive life
outcome, whether directly or indirectly.
To be accurate, research has yet to thoroughly validate which of the mentioned
qualities of humility indeed constitute the nature of humility and which qualities are
simply related to the disposition. This ambiguity is in part due to the lack of validated
humility measures (which I will explain shortly). However, drawing from these scholastic
conceptualizations as a starting point, I propose that a working definition of humility
could involve the following attributes: (1) an assured self-worth; (2) tendency to keep
one’s abilities and accomplishments in perspective; (3) willingness to acknowledge and
accept one’s mistakes and imperfections; (4) lack of self-deprecating tendencies; (5) lack
of self-enhancing tendencies; (6) willingness to improve on one’s weaknesses. One need
to note that humility should be considered as a dynamic construct comprising these
attributes, hence they may not be overlapping perfectly.
Humility: A Dispositional Character Strength
Many researchers have argued that one reason that humility is important is
because it predicts positive life outcomes (e.g., Emmons, 1999; Peterson & Seligman,
5
2004; Sandage, 1999; Sandage & Wiens, 2001; Worthington, 1998). There is evidence to
suggest that humble individuals are forgiving, compassionate, patient, and kind (Davis et
al., 2010; Exline & Geyer, 2004; Means et al., 1990; Powers et al., 2007; Peters et al.,
2011; Rowatt et al., 2006; Sandage, 1999). For instance, in a study by Exline and
colleagues (2000) it was found that people who were in a state of humility were slower to
retaliate in response to provocation on a laboratory task. This implies that humility could
positively impact interpersonal relationships. Further, the willingness to acknowledge and
accept one’s mistakes and imperfections (considered to be one of the attributes of
humility) may result in decreased justification of their actions. That is, they will not
blindly defend themselves and/or their actions. While defense and justification of one’s
actions will result in a perception of injustice, decreased justification will instead
promotes the seeking and giving of forgiveness (Exline et al., 2004). People who forgive
have even been shown to reap physiological benefits (Lawler et al., 2003; Lawler et al.,
2005).
There are also studies which found a link between humility and good academic
performance (Johnson, Rowatt, & Petrini, 2011; Rowatt et al., 2006). Though the
causality of this correlation is unclear, it is consistent with proposals that humble
individuals are open to novel ideas and have a strong desire to learn (Tangney, 2002;
Templeton, 1998). Being open and having a strong desire to learn is also a desirable trait
in the work setting (Reave, 2005). It is thus possible that humble people are well-liked
and respected whether in school or work setting, and that people are therefore willing to
assist them in times of need (Peters, Rowatt, & Johnson, 2011). Also, humble corporate
leaders who are resilient were also found to generate more profits for the organization
6
(Collins, 2001), while humility in employees was found to predict high job performance
(Johnson et al., 2011). Humility had also been shown to be beneficial in health and
therapy settings (Kurtz & Ketcham, 1992; Fontana, Rosenberg, Burg, Kerns, & Colonese,
1990). Though the causality of these findings is unclear, they could be tentative support
for the idea that humility is a resource that enables effective adaptation to life’s
challenges (Emmons, 1999; Exline & Geyer, 2004; Petersen & Seligman, 2004; Tangney,
2002).
Humility: Non-Defensive Response to Failure
The literature suggests that humble individuals adapt well to life events, whether
they involve mastering academic concepts, managing relationships, or handling work
challenges. However, navigating such life events is not easy because of the intermittent
failures one could encounter, and effective adaptation to challenges depends in part on
how the person responds to failures. Of interest in the current research is how humility
will influence the effect that failure could have on self-concepts. A body of research has
shown that failure can be damaging to a person’s self-views and also their subsequent
performance (Bronckner, 1979; Morrison, 1979; Nurius & Markus, 1990). Failure can
signal inadequacies in the self and some people react by affirming their positive selfviews whereas other might respond by wallowing in self-pity (Neff, Hsieh, & Dejitterat,
2005). If humility is associated with an assured sense of self-worth, what effect would
failure have on the self-concepts of the humble? This research examined how selfconcepts are activated during a failure encounter as a function of humility.
Failures, such as doing badly at a term paper or being rejected at an interview, are
inevitable in life, but people react to them differently. To cope with the ego threat that
7
follows failures, one can self-enhance by inflating positive self-views, deflating negative
self-view or both (Feick & Rhodewalt, 1997; Sedikides & Strube, 1997; Arkin, 1981).
For example, a student who has obtained an undesired grade may try to discredit the
negative feedback by deliberately attributing his or her failure to external factors (e.g., the
room is too noisy for me to concentrate) rather accepting his or her weaknesses (Blaine &
Crocker, 1993). The student might also try to undermine the failure by bringing to mind
positive self-concepts (e.g., remembering that he is good in sport) and rejecting negative
self-concepts (e.g., discounting the fact that he has been lazy; Baumeister & Jones, 1978;
Steele, 1988). Using these self-enhancing coping strategies, the student could disconfirm
the unfavorable feedback received in the failure, maintain or even inflate her/her sense of
self-worth, and deny personal weaknesses.
Self-enhancing is a common strategy to cope with failures, for instance, Steel
(1988) suggested that people may compensate for a failure experience by focusing on
their strengths. In a study by Baumeister and Jones (1978), it was found that after
receiving a negative feedback, participants gave themselves higher rating for selfattributes that were unrelated to the failure. It was also found that favorability of selfimage increased when participants encountered a failure in public (Greenberg &
Pyszczynski, 1986). This suggests that an overvaluation of self-image is a way to
compensate for failures. Besides these empirical support, self-enhancing defenses have
long been suggested by Adler (1956) who theorized that the strive for superiority is an
attempt to compensate for any inferiority.
However, there are grounds to propose that humble individuals are less likely to
employ such self-enhancing strategies to cope with ego threats. As just reviewed, there
8
appears to be consensus among theorists that humility is associated with an assured sense
of self-worth. Compared to the average person, humble people are more willing to accept
their shortcomings and to keep their accomplishments in check. This could suggest that in
the event of a failure, they have less compulsion to inflate their self-worth by magnifying
positive self-information and rejecting negative self-information to disconfirm the
undesirable failure feedback. This could stem not only from their secured sense of selfworth but also from their willingness to accept their weaknesses and having an openness
to correct themselves (Sandage, 1999; Tangney, 2002; Exline et al., 2004). Note also that
humble individuals are aware of their strengths. These strengths can help them to buffer
against failures in life. It is thus possible that during failures, humble individuals would
draw on their strengths and would be less averse to the negative feedback about
themselves, less likely to reject the negative self-information, and more likely to learn
from the experience (Tangney, 2002; Means et al., 1990).
Therefore, I predict that upon receiving negative feedback signifying failure,
humble individuals are less likely than non-humble individuals to over-activate positive
self-concepts and also less likely to de-activate negative self-concepts. This nondefensive strategy, if true, could yield psychological dividends for humble people.
Studies have found that people with inflated self-views are more socially maladaptive and
less liked by their peers (Colvin, Black, & Funder, 1995; Robinson, Johnson, & Shields,
1995). Further, research by Moore (2007) found that positive illusions correlated
positively with depressive symptoms. Extremely positive self-views have also been
linked to poorer self-esteem, well-being, and even academic performance (Compton,
2001; Robins & Beer, 2001). Hence, an contribution that this research could make for
9
future research on humility is in showing how the non-defensive response to failures
exhibited by humble individuals might facilitate coping with difficulties and excelling in
academic, relationship, and work challenges (Johnson et al., 2011; Rowatt et al. 2006).
Current Research and Methodological Considerations
The current research examined differences in the type of the self-concept
activated in response to a failure situation between humble and neutral (non-humble)
people. To simulate a failure situation, some participants were asked to do an arithmetic
task and were given feedback that they had done poorly. Other participants completed the
same task but were not given any feedback. Self-concepts were then measured. Note that
this research examined only global positive and negative self-concepts (good versus bad
self-related information) and not domain-specific self-concepts (e.g., intelligence).
Activation of global self-concepts was operationalized and measured differently in
different studies. In Experiment 1, they were represented by the number of positive and
negative traits of themselves participants can bring to mind. In Experiments 2 and 3, we
employed a latency response task to measure accessibility of global positive and negative
self-concepts. My over-arching prediction is that in the negative feedback condition,
humble participants would exhibit weaker activation of negative self-concepts
(Experiment 1: fewer negative traits listed; Experiments 2 and 3: weaker accessibility of
negative self-concepts) as well as weaker activation of positive self-concepts (Experiment
1: fewer positive traits listed; Experiments 2 and 3: weaker accessibility of positive selfconcepts) compared to neutral participants.
This prediction could be tested by measuring humility and examining how selfconcepts varied with humility. However, there are formidable validity issues with
10
available measures of humility. Self-report humility scales are not suitable for several
reasons (e.g., Ashton & Lee, 2005; 2007; 2008). Genuinely humble people would not
report their humble qualities as they are less attentive to themselves (Tangney, 2000;
Myers, 1995). Further, people could report themselves as highly humble, a desirable
quality, due to self-serving motivations (Johnson & Robins, 1993; Asendorpf &
Ostendorf, 1998). This critique should not be taken to imply that humble and non-humble
individuals never provide accurate scores of their humility. Rather it is the
acknowledgment that the current self-report measures are not able to provide an accurate,
holistic and reliable measure of humility and that users of the self-report humility
measures should be aware that such limitations. Further, an implicit association test (IAT)
of humility has also been developed to circumvent concerns associated with self-report
(Powers et al., 2007; Rowatt et al., 2006). 1995). However, this measure failed to
correlate with humility peer ratings and other humility-related outcomes, raising concerns
over its validity (Powers et al., 2007; Rowatt et al., 2006).
Therefore, I manipulated humility and tested differences in activated self-concepts
between humility and neutral conditions. However, past studies have not attempted to
manipulate humility. Humility could be manipulated by staging realistic situations or
having participants imagine themselves in suitable vignettes or videos. However, the
same situation or imagery material might not generate the intended response in all
participants; e.g., participants may feel low self-esteem instead of humility (HarmonJones, Peterson, & Vaughn, 2003). Implicit methods (e.g., subliminal priming) could be
used, but they tend to activate implicit representations, not necessarily conscious
11
experiences (Winkielman, Berridge, & Wilbarger, 2005; Yang & Tong, 2010), and my
aim was to examine conscious humility experiences.
The recall method was used, in which, before the failure simulation, participants
(in all three studies) were asked to recall an incident where they felt humble. Specific
definitions of humility were given to the participants such that they would not confuse
humility with low self-regard and humiliation. One other humility study by Exline and
Geyer (2004) also had participants recall humble experiences, although not for the
purpose of inducing humility. Recall is an effective method for inducing psychological
states (Gerrards-Hesse, Spies, & Hesse, 1994). There are concerns of memory
degradation with the recall method, but it avoids or minimizes the problems of other
induction techniques. Participants in the neutral condition were asked to indicate the
landmarks they encountered while commuting to school. They were not asked to recall
‘neutral events’ so as to avoid participants self-selecting the events and consequently
recalling events with varying emotional tone.
I also included a low self-esteem and high self-esteem condition in Experiments 2
and 3 respectively. Laypersons might equate humility to low self-worth, a trait
synonymous with low self-esteem. The notion that humility involves an assured sense of
self might make it seems too similar to high self-esteem. As such, humility might be
confused with both low self-esteem and high self-esteem. However, these constructs are
different and it is necessary to conceptually and empirically differentiate humility from
low and high self-esteem by showing that they produce different effects. Evidence in this
regard would assist in establishing humility as a distinctive virtue (Peterson & Seligman,
2004).
12
13
CHAPTER TWO
EXPERIMENT 1
Overview
In Experiment 1, participants were asked to list down traits about themselves after
the humility and feedback manipulation. Previous research found that in general, positive
attributes of the self becomes more accessible than negative attributes during failures as
part of the compensatory strategies to eliminate ego-threat (Wood, Giordano-Beech, &
Ducharme, 1999; Steele, 1988). Hence, it is possible that participants in both neutral and
humility conditions, not just the neutral condition, would bring to mind more positive
traits, and also fewer negative traits, when given negative feedback compared to when
they were not given any feedback. In addition, it is proposed here that humility weakens
the need to engage self-enhancing strategies to cope with ego threats. Hence, I predicted
that when given negative feedback, participants in the humility condition should list
down a lower proportion of positive traits (i.e., higher proportion of negative traits) than
participants in the neutral condition.
Method
Participants
Sixty-three undergraduates (52 females; M = 20.36, SD = 1.47) from the National
University of Singapore (NUS) participated for course credits. Participants were
randomly assigned to one of the four conditions in a 2 (condition: humility versus neutral)
× 2 (feedback: negative feedback versus no feedback) factorial design. See Table 2 for
cell sizes. Gender was not analyzed as a variable across all studies because of the
disproportionately lower number of males.
14
Procedure
The experiment was carried out in groups of 10 in partitioned cubicles to ensure
anonymity and privacy. Upon arrival, participants were informed that they would be
performing several unrelated experiments. The first experiment was presented as a “Scale
Validation Study” which was a cover for the humility manipulation. Participants in the
humility condition were asked to recall a humble experience. They were encouraged to
describe as much details as possible so that anyone reading their description could
understand their experience. Question prompts (“please indicate how you felt”, “how it
feels like to be feeling humility”) were given to facilitate recall. Participants were
instructed not to confuse humility with humiliation, feelings of shame, and low self-worth.
Definition of humility was also provided, detailing humility as being aware of one’s
weaknesses, modest of achievements, and acceptance of the fact that there are others who
are better. In contrast, participants in the neutral condition were asked to describe the
landmarks (e.g., stations, schools) they encounter while commuting to the school and
their daily routine on the evening before they go to bed. The purpose of getting the
neutral participants to describe two activities was to make comparable the time both the
humility and neutral participants spent on this recall task.
After the recall, participants proceeded to a task that measured the current
dependent variable. They were told that they would be participating in a “Conceptual
Thinking Study” which would measure their ability to think conceptually. Twenty
questions from the quantitative section of the Graduate Record Examination (GRE) were
presented using Medialab to the participants. The questions tested logical thinking ability
in a multiple-choice format and were presented one at a time. Writing materials were
15
provided for the participants to work out the answers. Participants were given up to 45
seconds to solve each question, after which the next question would appear on the
computer screen regardless whether they had indicated an answer. The entire task lasted
15 minutes, after which those in the negative feedback condition were told to wait for a
short while for their score. About a minute later, they were informed that they scored
below the 20th percentile of their undergraduate cohort. Those in the no feedback
condition were not told that their scores could be available and were not informed of their
results.
Next, all participants completed the trait-listing task. They were told to list down
as many positive and negative traits about themselves they could bring to mind at the
moment. The trait-listing task was self-paced with no time limit. Last, the participants
completed manipulation checks and demographics items before they were debriefed and
released.
Measures
Reported humility
As a manipulation check for the humility manipulation,
participants in all conditions rated, on a scale ranging from 1 (strong disagree) to 7
(strong agree), three items: “Do you think the situation described above reflects
humility?”, “How humble do you feel right now?”, and “How meek do you feel right
now?”. Scores for the items were averaged to produce reported humility (α = .90).
Negative feedback
As manipulation checks for the negative feedback
manipulation, participants in the negative feedback condition were asked to indicate the
percentile they scored. They also rated a perceived feedback accuracy item, “How
16
accurate do you perceive the score to be?” on a seven-point scale ranging from 1 (not
accurate at all) to 7 (very accurate).
Results
Manipulation Checks
Humility induction
An independent samples t- test conducted on the reported
humility indicated participants in the humble condition reported feeling more humble (M
= 4.80, SD = .45) than those in the neutral condition (M = 2.80, SD = .93), t(61) = 10.89,
p < .001, ηp2 = .68.
While these findings might suggest that the manipulation was successful, they
could still be of suspect considering the validity issues with self-report measures of
humility. As an added form of manipulation check, I coded the humility condition
participants’ recall descriptions for the extent to which they were consistent with current
conceptualizations of humility. By coding whether participants’ recall descriptions
coincide with theoretical conceptualizations of humility, I am assuming that the
description can be an indicator, albeit an imperfect one, of how much they had been
induced into a humble state. It is important to note as a caveat that not everyone who
writes about humility would feel humble. However, there is considerable evidence from
affect-induction research using the recall method that writing about specific affective
states is generally effective at inducing the targeted states.
Based on the literature review, six humility attributes were identified. They
appear to be common across many theoretical models and empirical findings (e.g., Myers,
1979; Hwang, 1982; Means et al., 1990; Clark, 1992; Richards, 1992; Halling, Kunz, &
Rowe, 1994; Templeton, 1998; Emmons, 1999; Exline & Geyer, 2004; Tangney, 2005).
17
First, humble individuals are thought to have an assured sense of self. This assured sense
of self-worth would entails having an accurate sense of one’s strengths and limitations,
not deliberately magnify their merits nor diminish their weaknesses (Means et al., 1990;
Templeton, 1998; Tangney, 2000). Humble individuals also have the tendency to keep
one’s abilities and accomplishments in perspective. This would mean not taking take
extra credit for accomplishments and not magnifying their strengths to inflate their selfworth (Means 1995, 2000; Peterson & Seligman, 2004; Exline & Greyer, 2004). Further,
they are willing to acknowledge and accept their mistakes and imperfections (Clark, 1992;
Emmons, 1999; Jennings et al., 2005). The lack of self-deprecating tendencies is also a
prominent attribute of the humble. For instance, Gibson (2007) found that individuals
who self-deprecate are not interested in having an accurate sense of self. This runs
contrary to what is known about humble individuals; their willingness to see themselves
accurately. In addition, humble individuals also lack of self-enhancing tendencies (Myers,
1995; 2000; Means et al., 1990; Richards, 1992). Last, the humble are willing to improve
on one’s weaknesses (Sandage, 1999; Jennings et al., 2005).
Every humility recall description was independently coded by two coders for the
presence (coded as 1) or absence (coded as 0) of each attribute. That is, each humility
description could obtain a minimum total score of 0 (i.e., it contained none of the six
attributes) to the maximum total score of 6 (it contained all attributes). Statements of the
six attributes as expressed in Table 1 were given to the two coders. One of the coders is
blind to the purpose of the experiment. Any misconception and disagreement about the
meaning of each attribute was first discussed and resolved. The coders then proceeded
with a few descriptions as practice. It was highly unlikely that any participants would so
18
clearly state that, for example, he/she had a sense of self-acceptance. The coders would
need to read the entire description and make an inference of whether, in this example, the
participant had exhibited a sense of self-acceptance in the situation he/she described.
Such coding is unavoidably subjective and hence the need for cross-validation between
two coders was all the more necessary.
The coding data, in terms of the percentage of descriptions containing each
attribute, are presented in Table 1. An intra-class correlation coefficient (ICC) of .84 was
obtained, indicating a high level of agreement between the two coders. Any
disagreements in coding were resolved and the final rating is presented in Table 1. Given
that it was not always possible to code an attribute from a description, and also that not
every attribute would be expressed in every instance of humility (e.g., the participant did
not have to keep his/her accomplishment in perspective because there was no
accomplishment in the described situation in the first place), we did not expect the
proportions to be too high. However, we conducted a one-sample t-test on the proportion
of each of the attribute against 50% and found that all but “Lack of self-deprecating
tendencies” is significantly above 50%. This implies that at least 50% of the participants’
recall descriptions contained these attributes. Further, assuming that a recall describing
humility should contained at least 50% of the humble attributes, I conducted another onesample t-test comparing the agreed rating against 50% (i.e., at least 3 out of 6 of the
humble attributes) and found that the recall descriptions contained a significantly higher
number of humble attributes, t(31) = 2.88, p = .007, ηp2 = .21.
Note that there was no data to compare these proportions with because the neutral
condition was asked about landmarks on the way to school which could not be coded. It
19
was therefore not clear whether non-humility descriptions would also contain these
attributes. This issue would be pursued further in Studies 2 and 3 where the proportion of
humility attributes were compared with that in two non-humility constructs, low selfesteem and high self-esteem, respectively.
Negative feedback induction.
Combining the humility and neutral
conditions, participants in the negative feedback condition on average indicated that they
scored on the 17.52th (SD = 2.94) percentile of the cohort. Hence, the negative feedback
participants in general were accurate in recalling their feedback. Also, there was no
significant difference between the humility participants (M = 17.25, SD = 2.69) and the
neutral participants (M = 18.0, SD = 3.46) in the negative feedback condition in terms of
the percentile feedback they indicated they received, t(24) = .65, p = .53, ηp2 = .57 .
Further, across the humility and neutral conditions, the average perceived
feedback accuracy score in the negative feedback condition was 4.88 (SD = .82). Noting
that perceived feedback accuracy ranged from 1 (not accurate at all) to 7 (very accurate),
this suggests that the negative feedback participants in general found the feedback to be a
moderately accurate. A t-test revealed no effect of condition on perceived feedback
accuracy, t(24) = .99, p = .33, ηp2 = .04 .
Main Analysis
The numbers of positive and negative traits listed in each condition are presented
in Table 2. In order to control for the differences in the total number of positive and
negative traits listed, the proportions of positive and negative traits out of the total
number of traits were computed and analyzed as dependent variables. As the proportions
20
for the positive and negative traits were opposite reflections of each other, only the
proportion of positive traits was analyzed and discussed.
A 2 (condition) × 2 (feedback) ANOVA conducted on the proportion of positive
traits (see Table 2 for the averaged proportions). The analysis revealed a significant main
effect of condition such that participants in the neutral condition listed more positive
traits than those in the humility condition, F(1,59) = 4.11, p = .047, ηp2 = .07. There was
a significant main effect of feedback such that participants in the negative feedback
condition listed a higher proportion of positive traits than participants in the no feedback
condition, F(1,59) = 5.53, p = .022, ηp2 = .09.
However, the expected interaction effect was not significant, F(1,59) = .003, p
= .95, ηp2 < .001. Despite the non-significant interaction, I proceeded with simple effect
analyses. Post-hoc analyses using pair-wise comparisons revealed that when given
negative feedback, participants in the neutral condition listed more positive traits than
participants in the humility condition, t(31) = 3.69, p = .001, ηp2= .31 (see Table 2). This
trend was not observed in the no feedback condition, t(26) = 1.42 p = 0.68, ηp2 = .07 .
Also, negative feedback led to a significantly higher proportion of positive traits than no
feedback, in both the humility condition, t(30) = 2.20, p = .04, ηp2 = .14, and the neutral
condition, t(29) = 3.29, p = .003, ηp2 = .27.
Discussion
Experiment 1 provided the preliminary support for the current hypotheses. After
receiving negative feedback, participants in both neutral and humility conditions listed
more positive traits and less negative traits, than without feedback. Hence, there was
evidence to suggest that both neutral and humility participants engaged in compensatory
21
strategies during a failure situation by bringing to mind more positive thoughts of
themselves. There was also some evidence to suggest that this compensatory effect was
weaker among the humility participants. When given negative feedback, participants in
the humility condition listed fewer positive traits than those in the neutral condition. This
pattern was not found when participants were not given any feedback. However, the
interaction effect was not significant, and the evidence presented here could only be
considered tentative.
Despite the promising results from Experiment 1 there was a need to replicate and
substantiate these preliminary findings. By asking participants to self-report the positive
and negative self-concepts they can bring to mind, the responses may be susceptible to
self-presentation motivations. Participants may not respond truthfully due to social
desirability or impression management. In Experiment 2, a response latency paradigm is
used to circumvent the limitation of using self-report. Also, by analyzing the response
latency of both the positive and negative traits separately I will able to examine the
differential accessibility of positive and negative traits. An additional low self-esteem
condition was added for comparison.
22
CHAPTER THREE
EXPERIMENT 2
Overview
Experiment 2 was conducted for three reasons. First, it aimed at obtaining
statistically stronger results. The second objective is to substantiate earlier findings by
measuring accessibility of positive and negative self-concepts using response latency (i.e.,
reaction time, RT). The accessibility of self-concepts in associative networks can be
measured by the response speed towards corresponding stimuli (Higgins, 1996). The
presence of a stimulus representative of a self-concept would be responded to more
quickly if the self-concept is more accessible. Hence, response latency provides an
indication of the accessibility of the associated concepts and affords an investigation of
the differential in accessibility of positive and negative self-concepts as function of
humility in a failure context. To this end, participants completed a task designed by
Markus and Kunda (1986) to measure accessibility of implicit self-concepts in which
they responded “me” or “not me” to a series of positive and negative trait words.
Differences in reaction time (RT) to the positive and negative trait words as a
consequence of the same humility and feedback manipulation would be examined.
The third objective was to differentiate humility from low self-esteem (LSE) by
examining whether they engender different accessibility of positive and negative selfconcepts in a failure situation. As noted, some dictionaries portray humility negatively by
equating it low self-esteem (e.g., Longman Dictionary). However, there are marked
differences between humility and LSE. LSE individuals have a tendency to compound
the effects of their failure by increasing negative thoughts regarding themselves and
23
overgeneralizing their weaknesses to unrelated domains (Kernis, Brockner, & Frankel,
1989; Brown & Dutton, 1995). They would not only accept negative feedback regarding
themselves in the event of a failure, but would also evaluate themselves very negatively
thereafter (Shrauger, 1975; Shrauger & Rosenberg, 1970; McFarlin & Blascovich, 1981).
In contrast, as argued, humble individuals might accept the negative feedback but they
should be less critical of themselves. Also, it is posited here that they are less likely to
trigger a high amount of negative self-concepts following a failure. To examine whether
humility and LSE produce different effects, LSE was also manipulated, using the same
recall technique.
Taking into consideration the findings from Experiment 1 and research on LSE, I
hypothesized that with negative feedback, the neutral condition should display the
shortest RT to positive trait words followed by the humility condition, and LSE condition
should exhibit the longest RT. A reverse trend was hypothesized for the negative trait
words; i.e., when given negative feedback, the neutral participants should show the
longest RT followed by humility participants, with LSE participants displaying the
shortest RT. In addition, it was hypothesized that participants in both neutral and humility
conditions would respond faster to the positive trait words, and slower to the negative
trait words, when given negative feedback compared to when they were not given any
feedback, whereas participants in the LSE condition would respond slower to the positive
trait words, and faster to the negative trait words, in the negative feedback condition as
compared to the no feedback condition.
24
Method
Participants
185 NUS undergraduates (135 females; mean age = 20.0; SD – 1.56) participated
for partial course credits. The participants were randomly assigned to one of the six
conditions in a 3 (condition: humility versus neutral versus low self-esteem [LSE]) by 2
(feedback: negative feedback versus no feedback) factorial design. Cell sizes are
indicated in Table 4. 9 participants failed the manipulation checks and were removed (to
be described further in Results).
Procedure
As in Experiment 1, participants completed all tasks in isolated cubicles. The first
task was the same recall task used in Experiment 1 but with an additional LSE condition.
Participants in this condition were asked to recall an incident which affected their selfesteem in a negative way. This was defined to them as an incident which made them had
negative evaluations of themselves and low self-esteem was further defined as a state of
worthlessness. As in the humility and neutral conditions, they were encouraged to
describe as much details as possible, and were furnished with similar question prompts.
They then completed the “Conceptual Thinking Study” as in Experiment 1. Next,
participants completed the self-concept accessibility task. Accessibility of positive and
negative self-concepts was measured using response latency. Participants were told that
their self-perception will be measured. The whole self-perception task including the
instructions was administered using the E-prime program. The instructions explain that
on each trial, they would be presented with a trait and they had to indicate whether the
trait was representative of them by pressing specific keys (one marked as self and the
25
other marked as “non-self”) on the button box. Detailed instructions were given to
emphasize the importance of responding as quickly as possible while being accurate with
their response. Participants were asked to place their left and right index finger on the
specific buttons before the task commenced. In this task, participants went through a
block consisting of 60 main trials for 2 times. The block consists of 20 positive traits, 20
negative traits and 20 neutral traits. The presentation of the trials within the block was
randomized across participants. For each trial, a fixation cross (+) was presented at the
centre of the screen for 1000ms, followed by either the positive trait, negative trait or
neutral word. These traits/words were presented for 3000ms. Response to the trait/word
was self-paced but if a response is not made after 3000ms, it would skip to the next
trait/word. A 1000ms break was given between each block. This task was adapted from
Markus and Kunda (1986) where participants need to respond “me” or “not me” to
stimuli relating to specific traits. Finally, the participants completed manipulation checks
and demographics items before they were debriefed and released.
Measures
Reported humility
Participants in all conditions rated the same reported
humility as in Experiment 1 (α = .83).
Negative feedback
Participants in the negative feedback condition were also
asked the same percentile question and rated the same perceived feedback accuracy item.
State self-esteem
Participants in all conditions completed a modified version
of state self-esteem scale (Heatherton & Polivy, 1991) consisting of performance and
social factors that are thought to be sensitive to changes in different aspects of selfconcepts. The scale using a five-point Likert scale that ranged from 1 (not at all) to 5
26
(extremely). The appearance component was removed because it was found to be
relatively stable in face of laboratory or academic failures (Heatherton & Polivy, 1991).
Also, this factor is irrelevant to the current failure design of the experiment.
Self-concept Accessibility Task
Using frequency of appearance, the traits
given by participants from Experiment 1 were ranked. Based on the ranking, I generated
a list of 40 traits (20 positive and 20 negative). The positive traits used were “easy-going”,
“thoughtful”, “knowledgeable”, “selfless”, “determined”, “hardworking”, “friendly”,
“patient”, “trustworthy”, “respectful”, “loyal”, “sociable”, “organized”, “helpful”,
“responsible”, “compassionate”, “strong-willed”, “forgiving”, “conscientious”, and
“confident”. The negative traits used were “tactless”, “disorganized”, “procrastinator”,
“immature”, “ lack of confidence”, “insensitive”, “cynical”, “un-forgiving”, “stubborn”,
“hot-tempered”, ”careless”, “selfish”, “messy”, “harsh”, “lazy”, “non-resilient”, “unmotivated”, “impatient”, “un-expressive”, and “lack of sympathy”. In addition, I also
generated a list of neutral words consisting of “structured”, “vertical”, “advanced”,
“horizontal”, “basic”, “circle”, “pointed”, “floating”, “curved”, “transparent”, “gravity”,
“translucent”, “frontal”, “concave”, “linear”, “fundamental”, “angled”, “cloudy”,
“dormant”, and “opaque”. This list of traits/words were then checked using the English
Lexicon Project (ELP; see http://elexicon.wustl.edu/) database to examine the lexical
characteristics. Only words that were fairly equivalent in terms of usage frequency and
word length were used.1
1
The three word types did not differ in usage frequency, F(2,57) = 2.58, p = .08, ηp2
= .08, and word length, F(2,57) = 1.79, p = .18, ηp2 = .06.
27
Results
Manipulation Checks
Humility induction
An one-way ANOVA revealed a significant effect of
condition on reported humility, F(2,173) = 17.50, p < .001, ηp2 = .17. Independent
samples t- tests indicated that participants in the humility condition reported feeling more
humble (M = 4.59, SD = .66) than those in the neutral condition (M = 3.16, SD = 1.27),
t(111) = 12.37, p < .001, ηp2 = .58, and those in the LSE condition (M = 3.21, SD =
1.12), t(116) = 9.48, p < .001, ηp2 = .44, and no difference between the neutral and LSE
conditions, t(119) = .21, p = .836, ηp2 < .001.
As in Experiment 1, the presence/absence of the same six humility attributes in
the descriptions was independently coded by two coders. Recalled descriptions from both
humility and LSE conditions were coded, so that the extent to which the attributes were
present in both types of descriptions could be compared. A high degree of inter-rater
reliability was obtained, intra-class correlation coefficient (ICC) = .93. As shown in
Table 1, the attribute proportions in the humility condition were comparable to those
obtained in Experiment 1. All of the attributes were significantly above 50%, implying
that at least half of the participants’ recall descriptions contained the attributes. In
contrast, none of the recall descriptions in the LSE condition contained any of the humble
attributes hence comparisons between both conditions were not possible. I then
conducted another one-sample t-test comparing the agreed rating against 50% and it was
found that the recalled humility descriptions contain at least 50% of the humility traits,
t(54) = 3.25, p = .002, ηp2 = .16.
28
Induction of Low self-esteem
A one-way ANOVA revealed a significant
effect of condition on state self-esteem, F(2,173) = 7.41, p = .001, ηp2 = .08 . Further
independent samples t- tests showed that participants in LSE condition (M = 2.69, SD
= .48) were lower in state self-esteem compared to those in the humility condition (M =
3.04, SD = .56), t(116) = 3.63, p < .001, ηp2 = .10, and those in the neutral condition (M
= 3.05, SD = .69), t(119) = 3.30, p = .001, ηp2 = .08. No difference in state self-esteem
was found between the humility and neutral conditions, t(111) = .07, p = .95, ηp2 < .001.
Negative feedback induction 9 negative feedback participants (3 from the
humility condition, 5 from the neutral condition and 1 from the LSE condition)
incorrectly recalled their scores to be more than the 20th percentile and were removed
from the analysis. All other negative feedback participants correctly recalled their
feedback score. Average perceived feedback accuracy score in the negative feedback
condition (across the humility, neutral, and LSE conditions) was 5.28 (SD = 1.10). Hence,
the negative feedback participants in general found the feedback to be moderately
accurate. A one-way ANOVA revealed no effect of condition on perceived feedback
accuracy, F(2.92) = .16, p = .85, ηp2 < .001 .
Main Analysis
Before analyzing the latency response, the concern of whether the number of self
and non-self responses differed across the six conditions and between the positive and
negative traits should first be addressed. Such differential responding could in turn
account for differences in latency responses. For example, slower responses to the
positive trait words by humility participants as compared to the neutral participants in the
feedback condition might not imply weaker accessibility of positive self-concepts in the
29
humility participants but could be due to the lower relevance of these positive traits to the
humility participants (put differently, the humility participants indicated less self
responses and more nonself responses to the positive trait words than the neutral
participants).
To address this concern, I subjected the mean number of self responses (i.e.,
positive-self and negative-self) in a 3 (condition: humble versus neutral versus LSE) × 2
(feedback: negative feedback versus no feedback) × 2 (traits: positive versus negative)
mixed ANOVA. There was no significant main effect of condition, F(2,170) = .38, p
=.68, ηp2 = .004, and feedback, F(1,170) = 1.00, p =.32, ηp2 = .004. The interactions
were also not significant: condition × feedback, F(2,170) = .16, p =.85, ηp2 = .002,
condition × traits, F(2,170) = .30, p =.74, ηp2 = .004, feedback × traits, F(1,170) = .007, p
=.94, ηp2 < .001, and condition × feedback × traits, F(2,170) = .34, p =.71, ηp2 = .004.
Note that nonself responses were not analyzed as they are reverse reflections of the
analyzed indexes. This indicates that participants across the conditions have similar
patterns of response tendencies. That is, the number of “positive-self” and “negative-self”
responses do not differ significantly between the conditions (refer to Table 3).
I also examined the response latency of self and nonself responses. Following
standard procedure in analyzing reaction time (RT) data, the raw RT data (in milliseconds)
was subjected to a logarithm transformation because of the positive skew in reaction time
data (Tabachnick & Fidell, 1989). All subsequent analyses were conducted on these
logged scores but the raw RT will be presented for ease of understanding. It was found
that self responses (across both positive and negative traits) were faster (M = 938.66) than
nonself responses (M = 1038.39), t(175) = 12.60, p = .007, ηp2 = .47. This finding is
30
consistent with past literature that negative or nonself responses will take longer (Fazio,
1990; Dodgson & Wood, 1998). Though the response latency of self and nonself
responses is significantly different, this finding do not pose as a problem because first,
participants do not differ in their tendency to respond self and nonself across conditions
and second, most participants did not select nonself to the positive traits nor self to the
negative traits (an average of 90% of the responses for positive traits was self and for
negative traits was nonself; refer to Table 3).
Next, I examined if the response latency differs significantly across positive and
negative traits. For this, I conducted a 3 (condition: humble versus neutral versus LSE) ×
2 (feedback: negative feedback versus no feedback) × 2 (traits: positive versus negative)
mixed ANOVA. The main effect of condition is not significant, F(2,170) = .36, p =.70,
ηp2 = .004 while a significant main effect of feedback was obtained, F(1,170) = 4.82, p
=.03, ηp2 = .28. The interaction of condition × feedback is significant, F(2,170) = 2.57, p
=.03, ηp2 = .19. Most importantly, the three-way interaction is also significant, F(2,170)
= 20.41, p < .001, ηp2 = .20. This implies that the response latency differs significantly
between positive and negative traits. This supports the previous finding that nonself
responses are significantly slower than self responses. As an average of 90% of the
responses for the negative traits were nonself and for the positive traits were self, the
response latency between positive and negative traits will clearly differs. As such, the
accessibility of positive and negative traits were analyzed and presented separately.
Accessibility of positive self-concepts
The logged RT for the positive self-
concepts were submitted to a 3 (condition: humble versus neutral versus LSE) × 2
(feedback: negative feedback versus no feedback) ANOVA revealed a significant main
31
effect of condition, F(2,170) = 23.54, p < .001, ηp2 = .22, a significant main effect of
feedback, F(1,170) = 6.11, p = .01, ηp2 = .04, and a significant interaction, F(2,170) =
13.01, p < .001, ηp2 = .13. Post-hoc analyses revealed that after given negative feedback,
participants in neutral condition responded faster (M = 836.1) to the positive traits then
participants in humble condition (M = 931.1), t(60) = 2.15, p = .04, ηp2 = .07 while the
latter responded faster to participants in LSE condition (M = 1201.8), t(61) = 5.09, p
< .001, ηp2 = .30. No significant difference among the conditions was found in the no
feedback condition, F(2,79) = .99, p = .37, ηp2 = .01.
Further analyses revealed within neutral condition, participants who received the
negative feedback responded faster to the positive traits presented than those without
negative feedback, (M = 1014.4), t(56) = 4.58, p < .001, ηp2 = .27. The same trend was
observed in humble condition; participants who in the negative feedback condition
responded faster to positive traits presented than those in the no feedback condition, (M =
1026.2), t(53) = 2.28, p = .03, ηp2 = .09. A reverse pattern was found in the LSE
condition such that participants who received the negative feedback displayed a slower
response to the positive traits than participants who received no feedback, (M = 1067.6),
t(61) = 2.55, p = .01, ηp2 = .10 (see means in Table 4).
Accessibility of negative self-concepts
The same analyses conducted on the
logged RT of the negative self-concepts revealed a significant main effect of condition,
F(2,170) = 27.59, p < .001, ηp2 = .25, and a significant interaction, F(2,170) = 7.40, p
= .001, ηp2 = .08. It was also found in negative feedback condition, participants in
humble condition (M = 1005.7) have a shorter RT to the negative traits presented than
participants in the neutral condition, (M = 1210.1), t(60) = 4.90, p < .001, ηp2 = .29
32
while participants in the LSE condition displayed the shortest RT, (M = 867.4), t(61) =
3.13, p = .003, ηp2 = .14. It was also found that the conditions differed significantly even
in no feedback condition, F(2,79) = 4.21, p = .02, ηp2 = .05. Pair-wise comparisons
revealed that participants in LSE condition (M = 963.5) responded faster to the negative
traits compared to participants in neutral (M = 1090.5), t(56) = 2.48, p = .02, ηp2 = .10
and humble condition (M = 1078.2), t(53) = 2.58, p = .01, ηp2 = .11.
Further analyses revealed that within neutral condition, participants who were
given negative feedback showed a longer RT than those with no feedback, (M = 1090.5),
t(56) = 2.77, p = .008, ηp2 = .12. No difference was found for those in humble condition,
t(53) = 1.52, p = .14, ηp2 = .04. That is, participants in negative feedback condition did
not respond significantly different from those in no feedback condition. Again, a reverse
trend was found within LSE condition such that participants who received negative
feedback responded faster to the negative traits presented than those who did not received
feedback, (M = 963.5), t(61) = 2.49, p = .02, ηp2 = .09 (see means in Table 4).
Discussion
Using a response latency paradigm, several results from Experiment 1 concerning
the differences between humility and neutral conditions were conceptually replicated and
differences between humility and LSE conditions were demonstrated.
First, as predicted, when given negative feedback, participants induced to feel
humility displayed lower accessibility of positive self-concepts, but higher accessibility
of negative self-concepts, than that of those in the neutral condition. These differences in
the accessibility of positive and negative self-concepts were consistent with the
differences in the proportion of positive and negative traits listed in Experiment 1. In
33
addition, as hypothesized, negative feedback increased the accessibility of positive selfconcepts relative to no feedback among the humility participants, but did not affect the
accessibility of negative self-concepts. These results suggest that failures would heighten
the accessibility of positive self-concepts in humble people but would not affect the
accessibility of their negative self-concepts. In contrast, the neutral participants
demonstrated both an increase in accessibility of their positive self-concepts, and a
decrease in accessibility of their negative self-concepts, in the negative feedback
condition relative to the no feedback condition. These findings are consistent with the
general strategy of self-enhancement following ego-threats in which positive selfconcepts are emphasized and negative self-concepts rejected (Sedikides & Strube, 1995).
The results revealed substantial differences between humility and LSE. Under the
negative feedback condition, the humility participants showed a higher level of
accessibility of positive self-concept, and a lower level of accessibility of negative selfconcept, than the LSE participants. Also in contrast to the humility participants, and in
direct opposite to the neutral participants, the LSE participants showed a higher
accessibility of negative self-concepts, and a lower accessibility of positive self-concepts,
following negative feedback relative to no feedback. The results suggest that LSE
individuals not only tend not to activate positive self-information in a failure situation
which humble individuals are more likely to do, they also tend not to activate the selfenhancing strategies typical of the average person when dealing with an ego-threat. The
results also indicate that the LSE participants exhibited higher accessibility to negative
self-concepts compared to the neutral participants in the no feedback condition, thus
34
suggesting that the arithmetic task alone (without feedback) may be threatening to those
feeling low self-esteem.
35
CHAPTER FOUR
EXPERIMENT 3
Overview
Experiment 3 has two objectives. First, it aimed to test the robustness of the
findings in Experiments 2 concerning the difference between the humility and neutral
conditions, using the same latency response task. Second, instead of a LSE condition, a
high self-esteem (HSE) condition was induced (using the recall method) so as to
differentiate the effects of humility and HSE. It is posited that humble people enjoy a
good sense of self-worth. However, HSE individuals are also known to have a positive
sense of self-worth and it remained a question whether humble and HSE would elicit
similar or differences responses to failure. It has been found that HSE tend to respond
less negatively to failures as they are able to recruit positive thoughts of themselves
following a failure (Dodgson & Wood, 1998). Further, they are more likely to employ
compensatory strategies such as discrediting the negative feedback or attributing failures
to external environment (Blaine & Crocker, 1993). In contrast, although humble
individuals are also aware of their strengths, they are just as likely to be aware of their
weaknesses. Hence, they will be less likely to resort to such compensatory strategies to
cope with a failure.
I hypothesized that after negative feedback, the HSE participants should display
the shortest RT to positive traits words, followed by the neutral condition, and the humble
participates should demonstrate the longest RT, and the reverse pattern was expected for
the negative traits words. In addition, I hypothesized that participants in all three
conditions (i.e., HSE, humble and neutral) would again respond faster to the positive trait
36
words when given negative feedback compared to when they were not given any
feedback. I predicted that both the HSE and neutral participants would respond slower to
the negative trait words following negative feedback relative to no feedback. No
prediction was made with regard to the humility participants’ responses to the negative
trait words as a function of feedback.
Method
Participants
133 participants (37 males and 99 females) from NUS took part in the experiment
in exchange for partial course credits. The average age of the participants is 20.3 (SD =
1.27). 6 participants were excluded as they failed the manipulation checks (to be
described further in Results). The participants were randomly assigned to one of the six
conditions in a 3 (condition: neutral versus humble versus HSE) by 2 (feedback: no
feedback versus negative feedback) factorial design.
Procedure
The procedure was the same as that of Experiment 2 except the change in HSE
condition. Participants in this condition were asked to recall an incident which lifted their
self-esteem. This was defined to them as an incident which made them had highly
positive evaluations of themselves. High self-esteem was further defined as feeling
successful and having positive aspects of themselves that they feel proud of. As in the
humility condition, they were encouraged to describe as much details as possible, and
were furnished with similar question prompts. They then went thru the same “Conceptual
Thinking Study” and self-concept accessibility task.
37
Measures
Reported humility
Participants in all conditions rated the same
reported humility as in Experiments 1 and 2 (α = .81).
Negative feedback
Participants in the negative feedback condition were also
asked the same percentile question and rated the same perceived feedback accuracy item.
State self-esteem
The same modified version of state self-esteem scale
(Heatherton & Polivy, 1991) was used an in Experiment 2.
Results
Manipulation Check
Induction of Humility
Reliability analysis conducted on the manipulation
check items revealed a high Cronbach’s α of .81. A one-way ANOVA conducted on the
reported humility indicated participants in humble condition rated feeling more humble
(M = 4.71, SD =.50) than participants in neutral condition (M = 2.98, SD = .67) and HSE
condition (M = 3.57, SD = .75), F(2,127) = 80.74, p < .001, ηp2 = .57. This indicated
that the manipulation was successful. Two coders (one of them blind to the research
hypothesis) coded the presence/absence of the same six humility attributes in the recall
descriptions. A high inter-rater reliability was obtained, intra-class correlation coefficient
(ICC) = .85. The attribute proportions in the humility condition were comparable to those
obtained in Experiment 1 and 2 (see Table 1). All of the humble attributes except “An
assured sense of self-worth” and “Willingness to improve on the imperfection” were
significantly above 50%. This implies that at least half of the participants’ recall
descriptions contained the other four attributes. In contrast, the humble attributes
proportions in the HSE condition were extremely low (< 5%), and also were so low in
38
variance that analyses on them would not be meaningful. I then conducted another onesample t-test comparing the agreed rating against 50% and it was found that the recalled
humility descriptions contain at least 50% of the humility traits, t(46) = 5.93, p < .001,
ηp2 = .43.
Negative Feedback
6 participants recalled their scores to be less than the 20th
percentile (1 from the humility condition, 3 from the neutral condition and 2 from the
HSE condition). The average rating of accuracy is 5.10 (SD = .98) which ranges from 1
(not accurate at all) to 7 (very accurate).
Induction of High self-esteem
Participants in HSE condition (M = 3.84, SD
= .74) indicated higher ratings of self-esteem than humble (M = 3.02, SD = .61) and
neutral condition (M = 2.89, SD = .56), F(2,124) = 26.14, p < .001, ηp2 = .30.
Main Analysis
A 3 (condition: humble versus neutral versus LSE) × 2 (feedback: negative
feedback versus no feedback) × 2 (traits: positive versus negative) mixed ANOVA was
conducted on the mean number of self responses (see Experiment 2). No significant main
effects of condition, F(2,121) = 1.60, p = .206, ηp2 = .03, and feedback, F(1,121) = 2.76,
p = .10, ηp2 = .02 were found. The interactions were also not significant: condition ×
feedback, F(2,121) = 1.29, p =.28, ηp2 = .02, condition × traits, F(2,121) = .03, p =.97,
ηp2 < .001, feedback × traits, F(1,121) = .09, p =.76, ηp2 = .001, and condition ×
feedback × traits, F(2,121) = 1.23, p =.30, ηp2 = .02.The results indicate that participants
across the conditions do not differ in their response tendencies. Again, consistent with
past literature it was found that participants RT for self responses were faster (M = 723.6)
than nonself responses (M = 819.8). And since the conditions do not differ in their
39
response latency, the difference in RT for self and nonself responses do not pose as a
potential artifact. In addition, most participants select self for the positive traits and
nonself for the negative traits (an average of 90% of the responses).
To examine if the response latency differs significantly across positive and
negative traits, I subjected the RT to a 3 (condition: humble versus neutral versus LSE) ×
2 (feedback: negative feedback versus no feedback) × 2 (traits: positive versus negative)
mixed ANOVA. There is no significant main effect of condition, F(2,121) = 1.59, p =.21,
ηp2 = .02. A significant main effect of feedback, F(1,121) = 5.05, p =.03, ηp2 = .04, a
significant condition × feedback interaction, F(1,121) = 3.75, p =.03, ηp2 = .06, and a
significant condition × feedback × traits interaction, F(1,121) = 3.95, p =.02, ηp2 = .06
were obtained. This implies that the response latency differs significantly between
positive and negative traits. As such, the accessibility of positive and negative traits were
analyzed and presented separately. The finding that nonself responses are significantly
slower than self responses was supported. And because an average of 90% of the
responses for the negative traits were nonself and for the positive traits were self, the
response latency between positive and negative traits will differs.
Accessibility of positive self-concepts
A 3 (condition: humble versus
neutral versus HSE) × 2 (feedback: negative feedback versus no feedback) ANOVA
revealed a significant main effect of condition, F(2,121) = 5.38, p = .01, ηp2 = .08, and a
significant main effect of feedback, F(1,121) = 37.74, p < .001, ηp2 = .24. Post-hoc
analyses revealed that after given negative feedback, participants in neutral condition
responded faster (M = 799.5) to the positive traits than participants in humble condition
(M = 908.7), t(40) = 3.65, p = .001, ηp2 = .25 and participants in HSE condition (M =
40
910.0), t(39) = 2.55, p = .02, ηp2 = .14. There is no significant difference between
humble and HSE condition t(41) = .22, p = .82, ηp2 < .001 and among the conditions
when feedback was not given, F(2,61) = 1.02, p = .37, ηp2 = .03.
Further analyses revealed that in all three conditions, participants who received
the negative feedback responded faster to the positive traits than those not given any
feedback, all ts > 2.32 and ps < .001 (see means in Table 5).
Accessibility of negative self-concepts
The same analyses conducted
revealed a significant main effect of condition, F(2,121) = 4.08, p = .02, ηp2 = .06, and a
significant interaction, F(2,121) = 4.78, p = .01, ηp2 = .07. It was also found in negative
feedback condition, participants in humble condition RT (M = 927.9) have a shorter RT
to the negative traits presented than participants in neutral condition, (M = 1184.9), t(40)
= 2.60, p = .01, ηp2 = .14 and participants in HSE condition, (M = 1163.4), t(41) = 2.48,
p = .02, ηp2 = .13. However, there is no significant difference between neutral and HSE
conditions, t(45) = .39, p = .71, ηp2 < .001.There is also no significant difference among
the conditions when participants were not given any feedback, F(2,63) = .13, p = .88, ηp2
= .004 .
Further analyses revealed that within the humble condition, there were no
significant different between participants who received negative feedback and those
without any feedback, t(44) = 1.39, p = .17, ηp2 = .04. Within both neutral and HSE
conditions, participants displayed a longer RT when given negative feedback compared
to those not given any feedback, t(38)neutral = 2.65, p = .01, ηp2 = .15, and , t(39)HSE =
2.81, p = .008, ηp2 = .17 (see means in Table 5).
41
Discussion
Experiment 3 largely replicated the findings of Experiment 2 with regard to the
differences between the humility and neutral conditions. Following negative feedback,
the humility participants showed lower accessibility of positive self-concepts, but higher
accessibility of negative self-concepts, than the neutral participants. As in Experiment 2,
the failure feedback increased the accessibility of positive self-concepts and decreased
the accessibility of negative self-concepts among the neutral participants relative to no
feedback. Also replicating Experiment 2, the failure feedback increased the accessibility
of positive self-concepts among the humility participants relative to no feedback, but did
not affect the accessibility of their negative self-concepts. Overall, the results support the
current contention that humility mutes the self-enhancing, compensatory mechanism
typically activated under neutral conditions in response to ego-threats.
The results also indicated significant differences between humility and HSE
participants. After negative feedback, humility participants exhibited higher accessibility
to negative self-concepts than HSE participants as predicted, though, there were no
significant differences between them in terms of the accessibility of positive selfconcepts. The accessibility of positive self-concepts increased in the feedback condition
relative to no feedback for both the humility and HSE participants, but it was only the
HSE participants who demonstrated weaker accessibility of negative self-concepts. These
results indicate that humility should also not be equated with HSE.
42
CHAPTER FIVE
GENERAL DISCUSSION
In the current research, I posited that humility enables non-defensive
response to failures. Existing findings suggest that humility is associated with an assured
sense of self-worth and a willingness to acknowledge and accept one’s shortcomings.
Drawing from this literature, I proposed that individuals in a state of humility are less
likely to cope with failures using self-enhancing strategies. More specifically, whereas
people tend to respond to by failures by emphasizing their strengths and rejecting their
weaknesses, humble individuals are less likely to magnify their strengths or diminish
their weaknesses. Therefore, I hypothesized that humility should lead to a weaker
activation of positive self-concepts and stronger activation of negative self-concepts
compared to the neutral state. Promising evidence was obtained that supported the
hypothesis.
Across three experiments, humility was induced by having participants recalled
personal incidents of humility. There is good evidence from the manipulation checks and
coding of the recall descriptions that this method was effective in inducing participants to
feel humble. In Experiment 1, a self-report listing method was used to measure activation
of positive and negative self-concepts. It was found that participants induced with
humility listed a significantly lower proportion of positive self-traits than those in the
neutral condition after receiving a negative feedback. This finding was conceptually
replicated in Experiments 2 and 3. Using a response latency paradigm in both
experiments, it was found that after receiving negative feedback, neutral condition
responded faster to the positive self-concepts than humble condition. This pattern was not
43
found when participants were not given any negative feedback. A reversed pattern was
found for the negative self-concepts such that while the neutral condition displayed a
longer RT than humble participants. Within both the humble and neutral conditions,
participants displayed heightened accessibility (i.e., shorter RT) to the positive selfconcepts after receiving negative feedback than when they were not given any feedback.
A reverse pattern was found for the negative self-concepts in the neutral condition such
that after receiving negative feedback, they reacted slower to the negative self-concepts
than when they did not receive any feedback. This was not the case for humble condition
which did not show any differences in the RT of the negative self-concepts regardless of
negative feedback.
To demonstrate the conceptual differences between humility with LSE and HSE,
the conditions were added to Experiment2 and 3 respectively. In Experiment 2 after
receiving negative feedback, the LSE condition showed the longest RT to positive selfconcepts among the three conditions. In addition, they also displayed a significantly
shorter RT to the negative self-concepts than both humble and neutral conditions even
without any negative feedback. Within the condition, after receiving negative feedback,
they responded slower to the positive self-concepts compared to no feedback given. In
Experiment 3 with a change of high self-esteem (HSE) condition, it was found that after
receiving negative feedback, neutral condition responded faster to the positive selfconcepts than both humble and HSE conditions. However, both HSE and neutral
conditions showed longer RT to the negative self-concepts compared to humble condition.
These were not found when participants were not given any negative feedback. Within all
three conditions, participants who received the negative feedback responded faster to the
44
positive traits than those not given any feedback. For the negative self-concepts, within
both neutral and HSE conditions those received negative feedback have a longer RT than
those without feedback. This was not found for the humble condition.
Reconciling the findings with Literature
Self-enhancing is a common strategy to cope with failures (Feick & Rhodewalt,
1997; Sedikides & Strube, 1995; Arkin, 1981). This strategy includes people
undermining their failures by reminding themselves of their positive traits (Baumeister &
Jones, 1978; Steele, 1988). This coping strategy was clearly displayed in the neutral
condition. After negative feedback, the neutral condition participants displayed
heightened accessibility of positive self-concepts and decreased accessibility of negative
self-concepts. Humble individuals, on the other hand, displayed such tendencies to a
significantly lesser degree. Compared to the neutral conditions participants, they
demonstrated weaker activation of positive self-concepts and stronger activation of
negative self-concepts. These findings appear consistent with existing profile of humble
individuals which construes them as less prone to brag about their strengths and
downplay their weaknesses in the presence of an ego-threat (Sandage, 1999; Emmons,
1999; Jennings et al., 2005). The findings are also consistent with a view expounded by
Carl Rogers (1961) that secured individuals are able to experience and accept every
aspects of themselves, both the good and bad, and feel less compelled to continually seek
the approval of others. Humble individuals are known to display qualities associated with
secure individuals, including an affirmed sense of self-worth and less defensive approach
in reacting to personal affronts. The current findings provide further empirical indications
45
that humble individuals are high self-acceptance and feel less need to protect their
identity or self-esteem when attacked.
After the negative feedback, LSE individuals responded slower to the positive
self-concepts (i.e., longer RT) than the neutral participants. Yet, a reverse pattern was
seen for the negative self-concepts; LSE responded quicker than neutral participants. The
findings in the LSE condition are consistent with the literature which supports the notion
that individuals with low self-esteem tend not to engage in self-enhancing strategies
following failures (Baumeister, 1982). Further, they are also known to overgeneralize
their weaknesses in response to failures (Kernis et al., 1989; Brown & Dutton, 1995). It is
notable that even without negative feedback, the LSE condition participants displayed a
heightened accessibility of negative self-concepts. This implies that the arithmetic task
itself poses as a threat, and may also suggests that LSE is associated a chronic heightened
activation of self-concepts under neutral conditions with no ego-threat whatsoever.
HSE individuals are considered to have a secured sense of self-esteem and past
research have shown that individuals with healthy self-esteem are more able to focus on
their strengths as an adaptive response to life’s challenges like a failure (Brown & Smart,
1991; Spencer et al., 1993; Dodgson & Wood, 1998). This was demonstrated in the
current research. When HSE participants were given negative feedback, they displayed a
heightened accessibility of positive self-concepts (i.e., shorter RT). Studies have also
found that individuals with HSE are particularly prone to engage in compensatory
strategies such after a failure (Wood, Giordano-Beech, Taylor, Michela, & Gaus, 1994),
probably more so than humble individuals. Consistent findings were found in Experiment
46
3; after receiving negative feedback HSE participants displayed a diminished
accessibility of negative self-concepts compared to humble participants.
Though the current method of failure manipulation has been demonstrated to
successfully induce feelings of failure (e.g., Dodgson & Wood, 1998), I do acknowledge
that having a negative feedback do not equate to experience of failure. However the
current findings seem to suggest that participants do experience failure (hence the
increase accessibility to positive self-concepts and the converse diminished accessibility
to negative self-concepts). Nonetheless, further studies can explore using an alternate
method of failure manipulation.
Further Implications and Conclusion
The idea that the trait humility bestows positive life outcomes has been
consistently suggested (Emmons, 1999; Exline & Geyer, 2004; Petersen & Seligman,
2004; Tangney, 2002). For instance, humble employees often demonstrate willingness to
learn and responding positively to negative feedback (Reave, 2005) which will benefit
their interpersonal relationships. Correlations have also been found between humility and
good academic results (Johnson, Rowatt, & Petrini, 2011; Rowatt et al., 2006). This
character strength helps one to adapt to life adversaries. As demonstrated in the current
research, humble individuals are able to take failures in their stride. Having an assured
sense of self help the humble to focus on their strengths in order to cope with the ego
threat. Yet, this focus is not an inflation of positive self-views. Further, they are willing to
accept their shortcomings and are not compel to reject negative self-information to
disconfirm the undesirable failure feedback.
47
Failures are inevitable in life and they affect some more than others. Humble
individuals are particularly not as affected by personal failures. Being able to accept
one’s limitations will oriented one towards learning and growth. This might suggests that
humble individuals have a higher subjective wellbeing than the non-humble people.
These predictive associations can be studied in future research.
As noted earlier, humility is a dynamic and possibly multi-dimensional construct.
Furthermore, the potential benefits of humility make this an important construct to
examine. However, the lack of a suitable measurement tool combined with the richness
of this construct makes it difficult to study. And because of this lack of theory-based,
reliable and valid measure, empirical research on humility remained limited. There is a
need to sharpen the measurement technique for which the field can be advanced. With a
reliable and valid measurement tool, the current results can be replicated. In the current
research, the recall method proves to be an effective way of humility induction but this
method does have its cons. Though definition of humility was given in the process of
doing a situation recall, participants can still hold a mistaken view of the trait. Further,
dispositional humility cannot be assessed with the current method as well. Nonetheless,
though the risk of having a mistaken view of humility exists, steps have been taken to
minimize this possibility. For instance, all recall descriptions were coded to check the
recall content coincides with what is known regarding humility.
Humility is more than a personality trait; it is a character strength that influences
how we cope with failures. Indeed, the three experiments support the notion that being
humble enables one to draw on his or her strengths to buffer against failures. Yet, these
strengths are not falsely inflated. Having an assured sense of self-worth means that these
48
humble individuals do not shun negative feedback, rather they would acknowledge and
accept their weaknesses.
49
Table 1
Percentage of Humility Traits in Participants Recall Descriptions (Experiments 1 to 3)
Experiment 1
(ICC = .84)
Rater 1
Rater
Agreed
2
rating
Experiment 2
(ICC = .93)
Rater Rater Agreed
1
2
rating
Experiment 3
(ICC = .85)
Rater Rater Agreed
1
2
rating
List of Humility Traits
1. An assured sense of selfworth (i.e., an accurate sense
of one’s strengths and
weaknesses)
53.1
56.3
56.3*
52.4
50.8
52.*
41.7
45.0
45.0
2. Tendency to keep one’s
abilities and accomplishments
in perspective
56.3
59.4
59.4*
63.5
58.7
58.7*
63.3
60.0
63.3*
3. Willingness to acknowledge
and accept one’s mistakes and
imperfections
65.6
59.4
65.6*
63.5
61.9
63.5*
53.3
56.7
56.7*
4. Lack of self-deprecating
tendencies
46.9
50.0
50.0
53.9
49.2
53.9*
60.0
66.7
66.7*
5. Free from arrogance
62.5
59.4
59.4*
68.3
71.4
71.4*
70.0
65.0
70.0*
6. Willingness to improve on the
imperfection
59.4
53.1
53.1*
50.8
63.5
63.5*
51.7
50.0
50.0
Average % of traits in recall
52.7
description
* percentage significantly above 50%, p < .001
54.6
53.7
62.0
56.2
60.1
52.3
52.2
58.1
50
Table 2
Proportion of Positive Traits listed across conditions (Experiment 1)
Humble
Negative
No
Feedback
Feedback
(n = 17)
(n = 15)
Proportion of Positive Traits Listed
M
.60
.49
SD
.14
.12
Neutral
Negative
No
Feedback
Feedback
(n = 18)
(n = 13)
.68
.19
.58
.20
Number of Positive Traits Listed
M
SD
8.18
2.56
5.67
2.13
10.0
5.11
9.30
4.17
Number of Negative Traits Listed
M
SD
5.82
3.04
5.53
1.55
4.17
2.00
5.46
1.57
51
Table 3
Response Tendencies: Mean Number of self versus nonself selections across conditions (Experiment 2 & 3)
Experiment 2
Humble
Neutral
LSE
Positive-self
NegativePositive-self
NegativePositive-self
Negativenonself
nonself
nonself
Condition
M
SD
M
SD
M
SD
M
SD
M
SD
M
SD
Negative
35.39 3.72
34.0
5.69
36.0
3.68 33.10 5.88
36.0
3.33 33.47
7.16
Feedback
No feedback 36.25
Condition
Negative
Feedback
3.43
33.95
7.06
Humble
Positive-self
Negativenonself
M
SD
M
SD
36.31 3.67 33.22 7.25
No feedback 35.0
4.80 32.29
Note that means are out of 40 trials
5.14
36.78
3.32
33.56
5.76
36.13
3.47
32.0
6.92
Experiment 3
Neutral
Positive-self
Negativenonself
M
SD
M
SD
34.55 6.24 31.75 7.14
M
35.71
SD
3.91
Negativenonself
M
SD
32.62
5.12
34.30
35.40
4.42
32.85
5.26
35.55
4.26
HSE
Positive-self
3.91
52
Table 4
Means of Reaction Time to Positive and Negative Self-concepts across conditions (Experiment 2)
Humble
Neutral
Negative
No
Negative
No
Feedback
Feedback
Feedback
Feedback
(n = 31)
(n = 24)
(n = 31)
(n = 27)
Positive Self-concepts
Reaction Time (milliseconds)
M
931.1
1026.2
836.1
1014.4
SD
166.4
125.8
132.7
160.4
LSE
Negative
Feedback
(n = 32)
No
Feedback
(n = 31)
1201.8
223.6
1067.6
147.6
867.4
177.2
963.5
154.3
Negative Self-concepts
Reaction Time (milliseconds)
M
SD
1005.7
166.8
1078.2
168.6
1210.1
136.2
1090.5
208.7
53
Table 5
Means of Reaction Time to Positive and Negative Self-concepts across conditions (Experiment 3)
Humble
Neutral
Negative
No
Negative
No
Feedback
Feedback
Feedback
Feedback
(n = 22)
(n = 24)
(n = 20)
(n = 20)
Positive Self-concepts
Reaction Time (milliseconds)
M
908.7
1070.1
799.5
994.2
SD
71.6
166.5
112.1
151.36
HSE
Negative
Feedback
(n = 21)
No
Feedback
(n = 20)
910.0
154.3
1043.6
198.3
1163.4
168.9
1000.7
198.3
Negative Self-concepts
Reaction Time (milliseconds)
M
SD
927.9
231.8
1032.5
218.7
1184.9
175.7
1028.4
183.9
54
References
Adler, A. (1956). The Individual Psychology of Alfred Adler. H. L. Ansbacher and R. R.
Ansbacher (Eds.). New York: Harper Torchbooks.
Arkin, R. M. (1981). Self-presentation styles. In J. Tedeschi (Ed.), Impression
management theory and research (pp. 311-333). New York: Academic Press.
Asendorpf, J. B., & Ostendorf, F. (1998). Is self-enhancement healthy? Conceptual
psychometric, and empirical analysis. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 74, 955-966. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.74.4.955
Ashton, M., & Lee, K. (2005). Honesty-Humility, the Big Five, and the Five-Factor
Model. Journal of Personality, 73, 1321-1353. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2004.09.016
Ashton, M., & Lee, K. (2007). Empirical, theoretical, and practical advantages of the
HEXACO model of personality structure. Personality and Social Psychology
Review, 11, 150-166. doi:10.1177/1088868306294907
Ashton, M., & Lee, K. (2008). The prediction of Honesty-Humility-related criteria by the
HEXACO and Five-Factor Models of personality. Journal of Research in
Personality, 42, 1216-1228. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2008.03.006
Baumeister, R. E, & Jones, E. E. (1978). When self-perception is constrained by the
target's knowledge: Consistency and compensation. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 36, 608-618. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.36.6.608.
Baumeister, R. F. (1982). Self-esteem, self-presentation, and future interaction: A
dilemma of reputation. Journal of Personality, 50, 29-45. doi:10.1111/j.14676494.1982.tb00743.x
55
Blaine, B., & Crocker, J. (1993). Self-esteem and self-serving biases in reactions to
positive and negative events: An integrative review. In R.F. Baumeister (Ed.),
Self-esteem: The puzzle of low self-regard (pp. 55-85). New York: Plenum.
Brockner, J. (1979). The effects of self-esteem, success–failure, and self-consciousness
on task performance. Journal of Personality, 37, 1732-1741. doi: 10.1037/00223514.37.10.1732
Brown, J.D., & Smart, S.A. (1991). The self and social conduct: Linking selfrepresentations to prosocial behavior. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 60, 368-375. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.60.3.368
Brown, J. D., & Dutton, K. A. (1995). The thrill of victory, the complexity of defeat:
Self-esteem and people's emotional reactions to success and failure. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 712- 722. doi:10.1037//00223514.68.4.712
Clark, A. T. (1992). Humility. In D. H. Ludlow (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Mormonism (pp.
663-664). New York: Macmillan.
Compton, W. C., (2001). Toward a tripartite factor structure of mental health:
Subjective well-being, personal growth, and religiosity. The Journal of
Psychology, 135, 486-500. doi:10.1080/00223980109603714
Collins, J. (2001). Good to Great. New York:Harper, Collins.
Colvin, C. R., Block, J. B., & Funder, D. C. (1995). Overly positive self-evaluations and
personality: Negative implications for mental health. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 68, 1152-1162. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.68.6.1152
56
Davis, D. E., Worthington, E. L., Jr., & Hook, J. N. (2010). Relational humility: A review
of definitions and measurement strategies. Journal of Positive Psychology, 5, 243252. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17439761003791672
Dodgson, P.G., & Wood, J.V. (1998). Self-esteem and the cognitive accessibility of
strengths and weaknesses after failure. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 75, 178–197. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.75.1.178
Emmons, R. A. (1999). The psychology of ultimate concerns. New York: Guilford.
Exline, J. J., Bushman, B., Faber, J., & Phillips, C. (2000). Pride gets in the way: Selfprotection works against forgiveness. In J. J. Exline (Chair), Ouch! Who said
forgiveness was easy? Symposium conducted at the annual meeting of the Society
for Personality and Social Psychology, Nashville, TN.
Exline, J. J., Baumeister, R. F., Bushman, B. J., Campbell, W. K., & Finkel, E. J. (2004).
Too proud to let go: Narcissistic entitlement as a barrier to forgiveness. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 894-912. . doi:10.1037/00223514.87.6.894
Exline, J. J., & Geyer, A. L. (2004). Perceptions of humility: A preliminary study. Self
and Identity, 3, 95-114. Doi: 10.1080/13576500342000077
Fazio, R. H. (1990). A practical guide to the use of response latency in social
psychological research. In C. Hendrick & M. S. Clark (Eds.), Review of
personality and social psychology: Vol. 11. Research methods in personality and
social psychology (pp. 74- 97 ). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
57
Feick, D. L., & Rhodewalt, F. (1997). The double-edged sword of self-handicapping:
Discounting, augmentation, and the protection and enhancement of self-esteem.
Motivation and Emotion, 21, 147-163. doi:10.1023/A:1024434600296
Fontana, A.F., Rosenberg, R.L., Burg, M.M. Kerns, R.D. & Colonese, K.L. (1990). Type
A behavior and self referencing: Interactive risk factors? Journal of Social
Behavior and Personality, 5, 215-232.
Gerrards-Hesse, A., Spies, K., & Hesse, F. W. (1994). Experimental inductions of
emotional states and their effectiveness: A review. British Journal of Psychology,
85, 55-78. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1994.tb02508.x
Gibson, B. (2007). The role of individual differences in sandbagging on selective
avoidance of self-evaluative information. Journal of Research in Personality, 41,
481-487. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2006.04.006,
Greenberg, J., & Pyszczynski, T. (1986). Persistent high self-focus after failure and low
self-focus after success: the depressive self-focusing style. J Pers Soc Psychol,
50(5), 1039-1044. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.50.5.1039
Halling, S., Kunz, G., & Rowe, J. O. (1994). The contributions of dialogal psychology to
phenomenological research. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 34, 109-131.
doi:10.1177/00221678940341007
Hareli, S., & Weiner, B. (2000). Accounts for success as determinants of perceived
arrogance and modesty. Motivation and Emotion, 17, 215-236.
doi:10.1023/A:1005666212320
58
Harmon-Jones, E., Peterson, H., & Vaughn, K. (2003). The dissonance-inducing effects
of an inconsistency between experienced empathy and knowledge of past failures
to help: Support for the action-based model of dissonance. Basic and Applied
Social Psychology, 25, 69-78. doi:10.1207/S15324834BASP2501_5
Heatherton, T. F., & Polivy, J. (1991). Development and validation of a scale for
measuring state self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60,
895–910. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.60.6.895
Higgins, E. T. (1996). Knowledge activation: Accessibility, applicability, and salience. In
E. T. Higgins and A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic
principles (pp. 133 – 168). New York: Guilford Press.
Hineline, P. N. (1992). A Self-interpretive behavior analysis. American Psychologist ,
47 , 1274-1286. doi:10.1037//0003-066X.47.11.1274
Hwang, C. (1982). Studies in Chinese personality: A critical review. Bulletin of
Educational Psychology, 15, 227-242.
Jennings, L., Sovereign, A., Bottorff, N., Mussell, M. P., & Vye, C. (2005). Nine ethical
values of master therapists. Journal of Mental Health Counseling, 27, 32-47.
Johnson, M. K., Rowatt, W. C., & Petrini, L. (2011). A new trait on the market: Honestyhumility as a unique predictor of job performance. Personality and Individual
Differences, 50, 857-862. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.01.011
Kernis, M, H., Brockner, J., & Frankel, B. S. (1989). Self-esteem and reactions to failure:
The mediating role of overgeneralization. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 57, 707-714. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.57.4.707
Kurtz, E., & Ketcham, K. (1992). The spirituality of imperfection. New York: Bantam.
59
Lawler, K. A., Younger, J. W., Piferi, R. L., Billington, E., Jobe, R., Edmondson, K., &
Jones, W. H. (2003). A change of heart: Cardiovascular correlates of forgiveness
in response to interpersonal conflict. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 26, 373393. doi:10.1023/A:1025771716686
Lawler, K. A., Younger, J. W., Piferi, R. L., Jobe., Edmondson, K. A., & Jones, W. H.
(2005). The unique effects of forgiveness on health: An exploration of pathways.
Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 28, 157-167. doi:10.1007/s10865-005-3665-2
McFarlin, D. B., & Blascovich, J. (1981). Effects of self-esteem and performance
feedback on future affective preferences and cognitive expectations. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 521 - 531. doi:10.1037//00223514.40.3.521
Markus, H., & Kunda, Z. (1986). Stability and malleability of the self-concept. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 858-866. . doi:10.1037//00223514.51.4.858
Means, J. R., Wilson, G. L., Sturm, C., & Biron, J. E. (1990). Humility as a
psychotherapeutic formulation. Counseling Psychology Quarterly 3, 211-215.
doi:10.1080/09515079008254249
Moore, D.A. (2007). Not so above average after all: When people believe they are worse
than average and its implications for theories of bias in social comparison.
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 102, 42-58.
doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2006.09.005
Morrison, S. M. (1979). The effects of success and failure on self-esteem. Australian
Journal of Psychology, 31, 1-8. doi: 10.1080/00049537908254643
60
Myers, D. G. (1979). The inflated self: Human illusions and the biblical call to hope.
New York: Seabury.
Myers, D. G. (1995). Humility: Theology meets psychology. Reformed Review, 48, 195206.
Myers, D. G. (2000). The psychology of humility. In R. L. Herrmann (Ed.), God, science,
and humility (pp. 135-175). London: Templeton Foundation.
Neff, K. D., Hsieh, Y., & Dejitterat, K. (2005). Self-compassion, achievement goals, and
coping with academic failure. Self and Identity, 4, 263−287.
doi:10.1080/13576500444000317
Nurius, P. S., & Markus, H. (1990). Situational variability in the self-concept: Appraisals,
expectancies, and asymmetries. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 9,
316-333. doi:10.1521/jscp.1990.9.3.316
Peterson, C, & Seligman, M. E. P. (2004). Character strengths and virtues: A handbook
and classification. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Peters, A.S., Rowatt, W.C., & Johnson, M.K. (2011). Associations between dispositional
humility and social relationship quality. Psychology, 2, 155-161.
doi:10.4236/psych.2011.23025
Powers, C., Nam, R. K., Rowatt, W. C., & Hill, P. C. (2007) Associations between
humility, spiritual transcendence and forgiveness. Research in the Social
Scientific Study of Religion, 18, 75-94.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/ej.9789004158511.i-301.32
Reave, L. (2005). Spiritual values and practices related to leadership effectiveness. The
Leadership Quarterly, 16(5), 655-687. doi:10.1016/j.leaqua.2005.07.003
61
Richards, N. (1992). Humility. Philadelphia: Temple University Press
Roberts, R. C. (1983). Spirituality and Human Emotion. Grand Rapids, MI: Wm. B.
Eerdmans Publishing
Robins, R.W., & Beer, J.S. (2001). Positive illusions about the self: Short-term benefits
and long-term costs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 340-352.
doi:10.1037//0022-3514.80.2.340
Robinson, M. D., Johnson, J. T., & Shields, S. A. (1995). On the advantages of modesty:
The benefits of a balanced self-presentation. Communication Research, 22(5),
575-591. doi:10.1177/009365095022005003
Rogers, C. R. (1961). On becoming a person. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Rowatt. W. C, Powers, C, Targhetta, V., Comer, J., Kennedy, S., & Labouff, J. (2006).
Development and initial validation of an implicit measure of humility relative to
arrogance. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 7(4), 198-211.
doi:10.1080/17439760600885671
Sandage, S. J. (1999). An ego-humility model of forgiveness: Theoretical foundations.
Marriage and Family: A Christian Journal, 2, 259 – 276.
Sandage, S. J., & Wiens, T. W. (2001). Contextualizing models of humility and
forgiveness: A reply to Gassin. Journal of Psychology and Theology, 29, 201-211.
Sedikides, C, & Strube, M. J. (1997). Self-evaluation: To thine own self be good, to thine
own self be sure, to thine own self be true, and to thine own self be better. In M. P.
Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology, Vol. 29 (pp. 209-269).
San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
62
Sedikides, C., Gregg., A. P., & Hart, C. M. (2007). The importance of being modest. In C.
Sedikides & S. Spencer (Eds.), The self: Frontiers in social psychology (pp. 163184). New York: Psychology Press.
Shrauger, J. S., & Rosenberg, S. E. (1970). Self-esteem and the effects of success and
failure feedback on performance. Journal of Personality, 38, 404-417.
doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1970.tb00018.x
Shrauger, J. S. (1975). Responses to evaluation as a function of initial self-perceptions.
Psychological Bulletin, 82, 581-596. doi:10.1037/h0076791
Spencer, S. J., Josephs, R. A., & Steele, C. M. (1993). Low self-esteem: The uphill
struggle for self-integrity. In R. F. Baumeister (Ed.), Self-esteem: The puzzle of
low self-regard (pp. 21-36). New York:Plenum.
Steele, C.M. (1988). The psychology of self-affirmation: Sustaining the integrity of the
self. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 21,
pp. 261-302). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Steele, C. M., Spencer, S. J., & Lynch, M. (1993). Self-image and dissonance: The role of
affirmational resources. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 885896. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.64.6.885
Tabachnick, B.G., & Fidell, L.S. (2001). Using multivariate statistics (4th ed.). Boston:
Allyn & Bacon.
Tangney, J. P. (2000). Humility: Theoretical perspectives, empirical findings and
directions for future research. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 19(1),
70-82. doi:10.1521/jscp.2000.19.1.70
63
Tangney, J. P. (2002). Humility. In C. R. Snyder & S. J. Lopez (Eds.), Handbook of
positive psychology (pp. 411-419). London: Oxford University Press.
Tangney, J. P. (2005). Humility. In C. R. Snyder & S. J. Lopez (Eds.), Handbook of
positive psychology (pp. 411-419). New York: Oxford University Press.
Templeton, J. M. (1998). The humble approach. Radnor, PA: Templeton Press.
Winkielman, P., Berridge, K. C., & Wilbarger, J. L. (2005). Unconscious affective
reactions to masked happy versus angry faces influence consumption behavior
and judgments of value. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 121-135.
doi:10.1177/0146167204271309
Wood, J.V., Giordano-Beech, M., & Ducharme, M.J. (1999) . Compensating for failure
through social comparison. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25(11),
1370-1386. doi:10.1177/0146167299259004
Wood, J. V., Giordano-Beech, M., Taylor, K. L., Michela, J., & Gaus, V. (1994).
Strategies of social comparison among people with low self-esteem: Selfprotection and self-enhancement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
67, 713-731. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.67.4.713
Worthington, E. L. (1998). An empathy-humility-commitment model of forgiveness
applied within family dyads. Journal of Family Therapy, 20, 59-76.
Yancey, P. (2000). Humility's many faces. Christianity Today, 44, 96.
Yang, Z., & Tong, E. M. W. (2010). The effects of subliminal anger and sadness primes
on agency appraisals. Emotion, 10, 915-922. doi: 10.1037/a0020306
[...]... commuting to the school and their daily routine on the evening before they go to bed The purpose of getting the neutral participants to describe two activities was to make comparable the time both the humility and neutral participants spent on this recall task After the recall, participants proceeded to a task that measured the current dependent variable They were told that they would be participating in a. .. of the self-concept activated in response to a failure situation between humble and neutral (non -humble) people To simulate a failure situation, some participants were asked to do an arithmetic task and were given feedback that they had done poorly Other participants completed the same task but were not given any feedback Self-concepts were then measured Note that this research examined only global... for the items were averaged to produce reported humility (α = 90) Negative feedback As manipulation checks for the negative feedback manipulation, participants in the negative feedback condition were asked to indicate the percentile they scored They also rated a perceived feedback accuracy item, “How 16 accurate do you perceive the score to be?” on a seven-point scale ranging from 1 (not accurate at all)... with a trait and they had to indicate whether the trait was representative of them by pressing specific keys (one marked as self and the 25 other marked as non- self”) on the button box Detailed instructions were given to emphasize the importance of responding as quickly as possible while being accurate with their response Participants were asked to place their left and right index finger on the specific... et al., 2004) Note also that humble individuals are aware of their strengths These strengths can help them to buffer against failures in life It is thus possible that during failures, humble individuals would draw on their strengths and would be less averse to the negative feedback about themselves, less likely to reject the negative self-information, and more likely to learn from the experience (Tangney,... of their undergraduate cohort Those in the no feedback condition were not told that their scores could be available and were not informed of their results Next, all participants completed the trait-listing task They were told to list down as many positive and negative traits about themselves they could bring to mind at the moment The trait-listing task was self-paced with no time limit Last, the participants... be relatively stable in face of laboratory or academic failures (Heatherton & Polivy, 1991) Also, this factor is irrelevant to the current failure design of the experiment Self-concept Accessibility Task Using frequency of appearance, the traits given by participants from Experiment 1 were ranked Based on the ranking, I generated a list of 40 traits (20 positive and 20 negative) The positive traits... should contained at least 50% of the humble attributes, I conducted another onesample t-test comparing the agreed rating against 50% (i.e., at least 3 out of 6 of the humble attributes) and found that the recall descriptions contained a significantly higher number of humble attributes, t(31) = 2.88, p = 007, ηp2 = 21 Note that there was no data to compare these proportions with because the neutral condition... which affected their selfesteem in a negative way This was defined to them as an incident which made them had negative evaluations of themselves and low self-esteem was further defined as a state of worthlessness As in the humility and neutral conditions, they were encouraged to describe as much details as possible, and were furnished with similar question prompts They then completed the “Conceptual Thinking... and disagreement about the meaning of each attribute was first discussed and resolved The coders then proceeded with a few descriptions as practice It was highly unlikely that any participants would so 18 clearly state that, for example, he/she had a sense of self-acceptance The coders would need to read the entire description and make an inference of whether, in this example, the participant had exhibited ... response to a failure situation between humble and neutral (non -humble) people To simulate a failure situation, some participants were asked to an arithmetic task and were given feedback that they had... getting the neutral participants to describe two activities was to make comparable the time both the humility and neutral participants spent on this recall task After the recall, participants proceeded... accuracy ranged from (not accurate at all) to (very accurate), this suggests that the negative feedback participants in general found the feedback to be a moderately accurate A t-test revealed no