Understanding how upward social comparison stimulates impulse buying on image-sharing social commerce platforms: A moderated mediation model of benign envy and self-esteem Article in Cur
Trang 1Understanding how upward social comparison stimulates impulse buying on image-sharing social commerce platforms: A moderated mediation model of benign envy and self-esteem
Article in Current Psychology · April 2022
DOI: 10.1007/s12144-022-03042-w
CITATIONS
7
READS
1,147
3 authors:
Van-Dat Tran
Banking University of Ho Chi Minh City, Vietnam
20PUBLICATIONS 362CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
Trieu Nguyen
National Kaohsiung University of Science and Technology (NKUST)
4PUBLICATIONS 19CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
Dung Minh Nguyen
National Kaohsiung University of Science and Technology (NKUST)
8PUBLICATIONS 101CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
Trang 2Understanding how upward social comparison stimulates impulse
buying on image‑sharing social commerce platforms: A moderated
mediation model of benign envy and self‑esteem
Dat Van Tran 1 · Trieu Nguyen 2 · Dung Minh Nguyen 3
Accepted: 23 March 2022
© The Author(s), under exclusive licence to Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2022
Abstract
The pervasiveness of image-sharing social commerce platforms has led to renewed interest in the topic of online impulse buying These platforms possess unique characteristics that facilitate both social comparison and impulse buying behavior However, little research has explored the relationship between upward social comparison and impulse buying in the context
of image-oriented social commerce Moreover, little is known about the underlying mechanism mediating and moderating this association Drawing on social comparison theory, this study developed and examined a moderated mediation model which integrates upward social comparison, impulse buying, benign envy, and self-esteem to address this gap The data was collected from 318 university students in Vietnam and analyzed using structural equation modeling The results reveal that upward social comparison directly stimulates adolescents’ impulse buying behavior, and benign envy mediates this rela-tionship Interestingly, self-esteem moderates the effects of upward social comparison and benign envy on impulse buying, respectively This study provides researchers with a new approach to explain online impulse buying on social commerce platforms In addition, our findings can serve as a reference for businesses and marketers to implement concerted strategies that encourage consumers to purchase impulsively on image-sharing social commerce platforms
Keywords Upward social comparison · Benign envy · Impulse buying · Self-esteem · Image-sharing social commerce platforms · Adolescents
Introduction
In recent years, the pervasiveness of social networking sites (SNSs), including Facebook, Instagram, Twitter, What-sApp, Pinterest, has made a significant change in the way people live, purchase, and communicate every day The growing popularity of SNSs has ushered in a new era of electronic commerce, known as social commerce, which is changing our thinking about online shopping (Xiang et al., 2016) Basically, social commerce is a combination of social media and e-commerce that conducts various types of social media-supported commercial activities (Liang & Turban, 2011; Xiang et al., 2016) Both consumers and retailers can benefit from social commerce It offers consumers a con-venient, seamless, and social shopping experience Social commerce also allows retailers to develop high relationship quality with consumers, resulting in increased sales and cus-tomer engagement (Oladapo, 2021) Thus, social commerce
is predicted as a potential research topic in marketing and information systems in the upcoming decades (Hajli, 2014)
* Dung Minh Nguyen
I108123114@nkust.edu.tw
Dat Van Tran
dattv@buh.edu.vn
Trieu Nguyen
F110121116@nkust.edu.tw
1 Faculty of Business Administration, Ho Chi Minh University
of Banking, Ho Chi Minh City, Vietnam
2 International Master of Business Administration, College
of Management, National Kaohsiung University of Science
and Technology, No 1, University Road, Yanchao District,
Kaohsiung City 82445, Taiwan, Republic of China
3 College of Management, National Kaohsiung University
of Science and Technology, No 1, University Road, Yanchao
District, Kaohsiung City 82445, Taiwan, Republic of China
Trang 3Emerging from Web 2.0, social commerce platforms (SCPs)
enable social interaction to assist online selling and buying
activities and allow consumers to review, recommend, share,
and purchase products or services (Hajli, 2015) Since 2006,
there has been a new emerging form of SCPs concentrating on
image orientation (i.e., image-sharing SCPs) The prominent
examples of image-sharing SCPs include Pinterest, Instagram
(in the US), and Mogujie (in China) Distinguishable from other
forms of SCP (e.g., text-oriented and video-oriented SCPs),
image-sharing SCP users share pictorial information rather than
textual information on SCPs, such as pictures about products
and lifestyles (Kim et al., 2017) However, just like with most
Web 2.0 applications, image-sharing SCPs focus on a particular
segment of consumers (e.g., females) or a particular product
(i.e., lifestyle and fashion) (Liang & Turban, 2011)
In recent years, image-sharing SCPs have deeply
influ-enced the users’ consumption behavior, especially in the
fash-ion segment Represented by Instagram, it has become the
biggest fashion source where the young are inspired by
vari-ous fashion styles from celebrities and influencers According
to a survey by FacebookIQ (2017), 56% of the surveyed social
media users consider Instagram as a major online platform
for fashion shopping, significantly higher than 42% of those
who cited Facebook Similarly, a recent report shows that
Pinterest users spend 80% more in retail than non-Pinterest
users (Burnie, 2020) Meanwhile, a recent report indicated
that customers tend to buy goods impulsively as 84% of
consumers have purchased products without any particular
plan, and approximately two-fifths of online consumption is
purchased impulsively (Saleh, 2017) In addition, for
image-sharing SCPs, impulse buying behavior is even more easily
simulated because “a picture is worth a thousand words”,
and the human brain can cruise through and process visual
information up to sixty thousand times faster than the textual
one (Tanda, 2017) The visualizing power of images has been
also proven to trigger viewers’ emotions effectively
(Messa-ris, 1997) These evidence suggests that image-sharing SCP
users are more susceptible to impulse purchases than users of
other social commerce platforms (Xiang et al., 2016) It is also
worth noting that among different consumer groups,
adoles-cents are prone to impulse buying (Liu et al., 2019) because
adolescents’ skills for controlling impulse sentiments are not
much dexterous, enabling them to try novel things and take
more risks (Pechmann et al., 2005) Thus, when surfing on
image-sharing SCPs (e.g., Instagram), the young are exposed
to a volume of visual information from vendors, news, friends,
celebrities, or experts, making it difficult for them to resist
the temptation of impulse buying Furthermore, adolescents
also represent a sizable online market segment owing to their
remarkable purchasing power (Statista Research Department,
2017) and heavy Internet usage (Liu et al., 2019)
Given the prevalence of image-oriented SCPs and the
importance of online impulse buying, understanding of
determinants of impulse buying on image-sharing SCPs has become essential (Liu et al., 2019) Extant literature has explored various factors that stimulate consumers to pur-chase impulsively on SCPs They can be categorized into four main types: social factors (e.g., social presence, similar-ity), website-related factors (e.g., social interaction, informa-tion quality), consumer characteristics (e.g., impulsiveness, usage intensity), and marketing-related factors (e.g., product availability, aesthetic appeal) (Abdelsalam et al., 2020) How-ever, to the best of our knowledge, understanding of factors influencing consumer impulse buying behavior in the context
of image-sharing SCPs has remained limited Xiang et al (2016) is a pioneer in studying impulse buying behavior on image-oriented SCPs that emphasized the role of parasocial interaction Most recently, Djafarova and Bowes (2021) iden-tified three antecedents of impulse purchases on Instagram: advertisement, micro-celebrity, and user-generated content
In addition to these variables, we argue that other factors still exist that could well explain impulse buying behavior
on image-sharing SCPs, yet have not been explored before
To address the above mentioned gap, this study focuses
on social comparison, which is a common social phenom-enon on SCPs, and explores its possible effects on impulse buying behavior The association between social compari-son and impulse buying is expected to be more prominent
on image-sharing SCPs rather than text-based or video-based SCPs due to its unique characteristics Besides that, the underlying mechanism of relationship between social comparison on image-sharing SCPs as well as impulse buy-ing and the correspondbuy-ing mediatbuy-ing mechanism (e.g., how this relationship occurs) and moderating mechanism (e.g., when the effect is stronger) remains inadequately explored Identifying these mechanisms will provide a comprehen-sive understanding of how upward social comparison on image-sharing SCPs stimulates impulse buying behavior Thus, this study attempts to enrich the existing literature
by examining the mediating role of benign envy and the moderating role of self-esteem in the effect of upward social comparison on SCPs on impulse buying
The contribution of this study is threefold First, the study is grounded on social comparison theory to examine
a model of impulse buying behavior in the context of image-sharing SCPs that had been rarely explored before Second, while benign envy was considered an outcome of upward social comparison, this research is a pioneer in examin-ing it as a mediator between upward social comparison and impulse buying Further, our study also sheds light on the moderating role of self-esteem on the effect of upward social comparisons on impulse buying Third, the findings from this study can be helpful for companies and market-ers to reconsider the positive aspect of impulse buying on image-sharing SCPs and thus formulate more effective strat-egies to induce impulse buying among online consumers
Trang 4Theoretical Background
Impulse Buying
Impulse buying was defined by Stern (1962) as an
unplanned, captivating, and hedonically complicated
pur-chasing behavior exhibited by consumers This buying
behavior usually occurs when the consumer has a sudden,
solid, and irresistible desire to purchase a particular product
immediately without considering the consequences (Park
et al., 2012; Chen & Wang, 2016), and the purchase
deci-sion is made in a short amount of time (Kim & Johnson,
2016) Notably, impulse buying possesses unique
charac-teristics that distinguish it from other purchasing behavior
types: unintended, unplanned, quick decision, inactive, and
thoughtless (Abdelsalam et al., 2020) The literature has
identified four distinct types of impulse purchases: pure,
reminder, suggestive, and planned impulse buying (Stern,
1962) Consistent with Stern’s definition, most users’
buy-ing behavior on SCPs can be considered impulse buybuy-ing
(Xiang et al., 2016; Abdelsalam et al., 2020) For example,
consumers browse images of clothes on SCPs purposeless
and then decide to buy a blouse or T-shirt could be viewed
as a purely impulsive purchase In contrast, planned impulse
buying occurs when consumers browse product images on
the SCPs with a prepared shopping list but only purchase a
particular product based on the offered discounts or
promo-tions A reminder impulse buying occurs when a user sees
an image of a cosmetic product and realizes that his or her
home stock is running low Meanwhile, after seeing a new
dress recommended by a SCP and purchasing it, it can be
considered suggestive impulse buying
So far, researchers have adopted many social and
psy-chological related theories to explain consumers’ online
impulse buying behavior in social commerce from different
angles The theoretical foundations for existing online impulse-buying studies are presented in Table 1 Accord-ingly, the stimulus-organism-response (i.e., S-O-R) frame-work are the most prominent theoretical approach over the past decade Impulse buying behavior in social commerce has also been explained with other theoretical approaches, such as flow theory (Hsu et al., 2012; Huang, 2016; Wu
Zafar et al., 2020a; Chen et al., 2016), social capital theory (Huang, 2016), social influence theory (Yang et al., 2021), uses and gratification theory (Zafar et al., 2020b) These diverse angles have offered a more in-depth and compre-hensive understanding of consumers’ online impulse buying
on social commerce platforms However, since the focus of this study is on image-sharing SCPs, which easily stimulate social interaction and comparison between users, we attempt
to explain impulse buying under the perspective of social comparison using the social comparison theory
Social Comparison Theory
Initially introduced by Festinger (1954), social comparison theory is a well-known psychological theory in explaining people’s tendency to compare themselves to others The theory suggests that individuals have an innate drive to accurately evaluate their opinions and abilities by comparing themselves to others in the social world (Festinger, 1954; Liu
et al., 2019) Based on social comparison theory, plenty of empirical studies have been conducted to offer insights into three aspects of social comparison, including comparison target selection (i.e., whom individuals choose to compare themselves to), comparison motivation (i.e., why individuals engage in social comparison), and comparison reaction (i.e., how individuals react to social comparison) (Gerber et al., 2018; Nakayama et al., 2018)
Table 1 Theoretical foundation
of studies on impulse buying on
social commerce
Theoretical foundation Study Cognitive emotion theory Verhagen and Van Dolen ( 2011 ) Flow theory Wu et al ( 2020 ); Huang ( 2016 ); Hsu et al ( 2012 );
Latent state-trait theory Zafar et al ( 2021 ); Chen et al ( 2016 ); Wells et al ( 2011 ) Parasocial interaction theory Vazquez et al ( 2020 )
Reflective–impulsive model Shen and Khalifa ( 2012 ) S–O–R framework Ming et al ( 2021 ); Zafar et al ( 2020a ); Zheng et al ( 2019 ); Chen and
Yao ( 2018 ); Leong et al ( 2018 ); Xiang et al ( 2016 ); Zhang et al ( 2014 ); Floh and Madlberger ( 2013 ); Shen and Khalifa ( 2012 ) Social capital theory Huang ( 2016 )
Social influence theory Yang et al ( 2021 ) Social impact theory Chuang et al ( 2015 ) Social network paradigm Husnain et al ( 2016 ) Uses and gratification theory Zafar et al ( 2020b )
Trang 5Regarding comparison target selection, individuals tend
to compare themselves with those who have similarities
(Buunk & Gibbons, 2007; Li, 2018) In general,
schol-ars have identified two main types of social comparison:
upward social comparison and downward social comparison
Upward social comparison occurs when individuals compare
themselves with others who are superior or better than they
are In contrast, downward social comparison occurs when
people compare themselves with others who are worse than
themselves (Liu et al., 2019) A recent meta-analysis study
on the social comparison (Gerber et al., 2018) reports that
individuals generally prefer making upward comparisons to
downward comparisons While upward social comparison
can lower self-regard (Tesser et al., 1988), Collins (2000)
argued that this was not always the case In many situations,
individuals compare themselves to others who are
bet-ter than themselves to obtain more useful information for
evaluating themselves (Buunk & Gibbons, 2007; Festinger,
1954), developing a positive self-image (Park & Park, 2017),
or creating a sense of belonging to a superior group and
enhance their perception of individual worth (Collins, 2000)
With the recent emergence of image-sharing SCPs, social
comparison has become more ubiquitous We find several
features on image-sharing SCPs that facilitate upward
social comparison First, celebrities and influencers often
share relevant images of sponsored products and services
onto image-sharing SCPs for advertising purposes Users,
thereby, can quickly come across these perfect and beautiful
images that stimulate them to make comparisons Second,
image-sharing SCPs operate on SNSs with many
commer-cial activities while SNSs are a fertile ground for upward
social comparison (Taylor & Strutton, 2016) Third, with
increasing time for using SNSs, users stand a higher chance
of approaching comparison-making stimulus on
image-shar-ing SCPs through a glimpse at advertisements (Liu et al.,
2019) Therefore, upward social comparison on
image-shar-ing SCPs may happen when users look at captivatimage-shar-ing images
of online celebrities in terms of their superior possessions
and appearance
Research Model and Hypotheses Development
We build on the social comparison theory to propose the research model (see Fig. 1) consisting of four constructs The model explains how impulse buying behavior on image-sharing SCPs is influenced
by upward social comparison, benign envy, and self-esteem
Upward Social Comparison, Benign Envy, and Impulse Buying
Existing literature on social comparison has identified two opposing types of consequences from upward social com-parison On the one hand, when individuals compare them-selves with others who are better than themthem-selves, they will feel inferior about themselves (de Vries & Kühne, 2015), leading to some negative emotions or outcomes, such as depression (Li, 2018), mental health damage (Jang et al., 2016) On the other hand, upward social comparison can also bring positive benefits, such as enhancing job per-formance (Cadsby et al., 2019) or eliciting inspiration on SNSs (Meier & Schäfer, 2018) Notably, some scholars have recently paid much attention to a common post-comparison emotion on SNSs, namely envy (Ahn et al., 2021; de Vries
& Kühne, 2015), which is defined as a painful experience when facing the good fortune of others (Tai et al., 2012)
In general, there are two types of envy: malicious envy and benign envy (Smith & Kim, 2007) Malicious envy views envy from a negative perspective that may carry harmful consequences, such as burnout (Liu & Ma, 2018) or fear of missing out (Yin et al., 2019), while benign envy considers envy from a positive perspective Although malicious envy
is the traditional and dominant approach, benign envy has gained more attention from scholars in recent studies due to its positive nature The latter approach emphasizes how envy can motivate individuals to excel and achieve desired things
by improving themselves, rather than bringing the envied targets down (Li, 2018; Crusius & Lange, 2014)
Fig 1 The research model
Trang 6The logic mentioned above can be applied in the current study
When making social comparison, people realize that they lack
and desire something others have, such as possessions, superior
qualities, or achievements, leading to feeling of envy (Li, 2018)
Prior studies have concurred that envy often comes from the
upward social comparison (Ahn et al., 2021; Lange & Crusius,
2015; Li, 2018; Zheng et al., 2018) Suls et al (2002) found that
making a social comparison is a way to fulfill their need for
self-enhancement In the context of image-sharing SCPs, the
appeal-ing images of products and appearance of other users, especially
celebrities and influencers, are the key to intuitively arousing
upward social comparison After making upward social
com-parison with images of deemed superior celebrities, consumers
may form a sense of benign envy that spurs them to take action
in self-improvement to obtain the desired thing similar to that
possessed by the compared one (Crusius & Mussweiler, 2012;
to get caught up in thoughtless behaviors (i.e., impulse buying)
to get the desired items quickly The effect of benign envy on a
desire to buy luxury fashion items has been validated in a recent
study by Loureiro et al (2020) Accordingly, we hypothesize that:
H1 Upward social comparison on image-sharing SCPs
is positively associated with benign envy.
H2 Benign envy is positively associated with impulse
buying on image-sharing SCPs.
The social comparison theory indicates that individuals tend
to evaluate themselves by comparing with others about owned
assets or consumption behaviors (Festinger, 1954) When
brows-ing SCPs, users are immersed in a colossal amount of information
and pictures from other users, especially from celebrities and
influ-encers on online advertisements (Xiang et al., 2016; Abdelsalam
com-parison with the advertising pictures of celebrities or influencers
on image-sharing SCPs would provoke cravings for desired
prod-ucts Hence, we argue that users are easily stimulated by the visual
appeal to make social comparisons and then purchase products
impulsively The association between upward social comparison
and impulse buying has been validated in some contexts, such as
SNSs (Liu et al., 2019) and superior goods (Crusius & Mussweiler,
2012) Accordingly, the following hypothesis is proposed:
H3 Upward social comparison on image-sharing SCPs
is positively associated with impulse buying
The Moderating Role of Self‑esteem
Although upward social comparison and benign envy are
expected to impact impulse buying behavior positively, the
impacts may not be the same among adolescents Some
individ-ual characteristics may moderate these effects, for example,
self-esteem Self-esteem refers to an individual’s subjective evaluation
of their worth and denotes how individuals feel about themselves (Rosenberg, 1965; Wang et al., 2017) Self-esteem is an attrac-tive psychological construct since it is used to predict or explain certain outcomes, such as counterproductive working behaviors (Whelpley & McDaniel, 2016), fashion clothing involvement (Khare et al., 2012), and depression (Zhou et al., 2020)
In addition, past research has been attentive to the importance
of self-esteem degree (i.e., low self-esteem and high self-esteem) and empirically indicated that self-esteem exerts a moderating role between some psychological variables (e.g., Ahmad & Begum, 2020; Lee et al., 2018; Wang et al., 2018a, ) For example, Wang
was stronger for SNSs users with low self-esteem than those with high self-esteem Some scholars posited that the lower self-esteem users seem to feel satisfied in life and benefit more from their use of Facebook than the higher self-esteem users (Chen & Bello, 2017; Lee et al., 2018) Regarding image-sharing SCPs, little research has explored the moderating role of self-esteem on the indirect relations between upward social comparison and users’ impulse buying behav-ior Verplanken et al (2005) posited that low self-esteem individuals find impulse buying difficult to avoid while Dhandra (2020) sug-gested that low self-esteem individuals are fall victim to impulsivity Therefore, we predict that when making social comparisons with the superior on image-sharing SCPs, low self-esteem consumers are more susceptible to impulse buying, compared to high self-esteem ones Additionally, when the sense of benign envy occurs after mak-ing the upward social comparison, lower self-esteem individuals are more likely to purchase goods impulsively to quell the gap with the compared people Accordingly, we propose the following hypotheses:
H4a Self-esteem negatively moderates the relationship
between upward social comparison and impulse buying on image-sharing SCPs
H4b Self-esteem negatively moderates the relationship
between benign envy and impulse buying on image-sharing SCPs
Research Methodology
Sample and Data Collection
The research model was tested using data collected from stu-dents in the three most prominent universities in the south of Vietnam through convenience sampling method The partici-pants were asked to complete questionnaires provided directly by the authors The college students were chosen as respondents for two main reasons First, prior scholars argued that adolescents are more impulsive than adults and children (Pechmann et al., 2005) Second, teenagers tend to spend more time on social media compared to children and adults (Ballard, 2019) There-fore, the image or information presented on SCPs may offer ado-lescents ample opportunities to make social comparisons More-over, many prior studies have suggested that it is appropriate to use college student samples in online impulse buying research
Trang 7because young people are the predominant online consumers
(e.g., Liu et al., 2019; Chan et al., 2017; Abdelsalam et al., 2020)
Before proceeding to the official survey, the pilot study was
con-ducted with 50 students to determine the feasibility of this research
The Cronbach’s alpha values of all constructs in the pilot test were
higher than 0.7 (e.g., 0.916 for upward social comparison, 0.907 for
impulse buying, 0.921 for benign envy, and 0.930 for self-esteem)
The results demonstrate that the questionnaire is reliable The official
survey was conducted in ten weeks, from March to May 2020 A total
of 361 students completed the survey questionnaire After
remov-ing 43 invalid responses, the final sample included 318 respondents
Among them, 141 (44.3% are male) and 177 (55.7%) are female
Respondents’ ages range between 18 and 24 Besides, 67
respond-ents (21.1%) are freshman, 112 (35.2%) are second-year studrespond-ents, 95
(29.8%) are third-year students and 44 (13.9%) are senior More than
half of surveyed students (51.9%) spent from 1 to 3 h per day, and
only 18.9% of respondents spent less than 1 h per day using SCPs
Measurement
The present research model consisted of four latent constructs,
in which each construct was measured with multiple items The
measurement items used in this research were adapted from
rel-evant studies discussed in the foregoing section Accordingly, the
scale for upward social comparison on image-sharing SCPs was
measured with six items adapted from Bai et al (2013) Benign
envy scale was assessed with five items adapted from Lange and
Crusius (2015) Self-esteem scale was adopted from Rosenberg
(1965) with ten items Impulse buying was measured with eight
items adapted from Badgaiyan et al (2016) and Verplanken and
Herabadi (2001) All measurement items were rated on a
seven-point Likert scale (i.e., 1 = strongly disagree; 7 = strongly agree)
and provided in Appendix A, Table 6
Control Variables
Prior studies have suggested age, gender, and monthly living
expenditure were related to students’ impulse buying behaviors
(Vohs & Faber, 2007) Additionally, Arampatzi et al (2018)
dem-onstrated that the amount of time on SNSs significantly influenced
students’ well-being Thus, this study included gender and amount
of time on SCPs per day as two control variables in the research
model to limit their confounding effects on the dependent variable
Results
We utilized structural equation modeling (SEM) as the main
data analysis method We followed the two-step approach
(Anderson & Gerbing, 1988) to test the measurement model
and structural model
Measurement Model
Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was performed to evaluate the measurement model’s overall fit, reliabil-ity, convergent validreliabil-ity, and discriminant validity using AMOS software First, the goodness of fit is evaluated via six indexes suggested by Bagozzi and Yi (1988), including
index (GFI > 0.90), adjusted goodness of fit index (AGFI
>0.90); normed fit index (NFI > 0.90), comparative fit index (CFI > 0.90), and root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA <0.08) As shown in Table 2, all goodness-of-fit indices exceed the recommended cut-off values, indicating that the measurement model is relevant to the collected data Second, Cronbach’s alpha (CA) and composite reliability (CR) were employed to examine the reliability of all vari-ables As illustrated in Table 2, the CA and CR values are higher than the recommended threshold of 0.70 (Hair et al., 2010) Thus, the reliability of all variable is satisfied Third, the convergent validity was evaluated by two criteria, including (1) average variance extracted (AVE) of variables and (2) factor loadings index Table 2 shows that the AVE for all variables is greater than 0.50 (Fornell & Larcker, 1981), and all factor loadings are significantly greater than 0.60 (Hair et al., 2010), supporting convergent validity of the instrument Fourth, the discriminant validity is evaluated by compar-ing the AVE with the square of the correlation parameters
As shown in Table 3, the AVE values of each construct are higher than the square of correlations between that and any other construct, demonstrating a solid discriminant validity (Fornell & Larcker, 1981) In addition, the confidence inter-val of correlation coefficients does not include the inter-value of
1, confirming sufficient discriminant validity
Because this study used the self-reported survey to collect data, the likelihood of common method variance (CMV) is a concern Following suggestions of Podsakoff
et al (2012), we took more efforts to minimize the effects
of CMV by guaranteeing the respondent anonymity and pre-testing the scale items adapted from previous stud-ies In addition, we performed Harman’s one-factor test
to examine the severity of CMV As a result, the explana-tory variance for the first factor is 33.87%, lower than the threshold of 50% (Podsakoff et al., 2012) Therefore, the problem of CMV is not serious in this study
Structural Model
df (150) = 2.521, RMSEA = 0.069, NFI = 0.917, RFI = 0.905, GFI = 0.900, IFI = 0.948, TLI = 0.941 and CFI = 0.948 These indices are acceptable (Bagozzi & Yi, 1988), indicating that the structural model achieves overall fit
Trang 8The results of the structural model are provided in Table 4
and Fig. 2 Accordingly, upward social comparison on SCPs
has significant and positive effect on benign envy
(coeffi-cient path = 0.727, p < 0.001) and impulse buying (coeffi(coeffi-cient
path = 0.386, p < 0.001), supporting H1 and H3 In addition, the
influence of benign envy on impulse buying is significant and
positive (coefficient path = 0.532, p < 0.001), confirming H2
Figure 2 also shows the values of explained variances for benign
demonstrate that the model achieves good explanatory power Besides that, to identify whether the inclusion of the control variables (i.e., gender, usage time on SCPs per day) influences interpretation of the results of the proposed model (Spector & Brannick, 2011), we conducted a SEM analysis using bootstrap 2.000 re-samples The two control variables were inserted between upward social comparison on SCPs and impulse buying to inves-tigate whether these relationships can still be supported with the effect of the control variables As a result, all hypotheses are still supported when adding the two control variables Thus, the control variables do not have a bias towards the current outcomes
Mediating Effect of Benign Envy
To examine the mediating role of benign envy in the asso-ciation between upward social comparison on SCPs and impulse buying, we used model 4 of PROCESS macro (Hayes, 2013; Hayes et al., 2017) and bootstrapping method
Table 2 Reliability and
convergent validity of the
measurement model
CR = Composite reliability; AVE = Average variance extracted, ***: p < 0.001
Goodness-of- fit indexes: χ 2 /df (371) = 1.220, RMSEA = 0.026, CFI = 0.988, NFI = 0.938, GFI = 0.911, AGFI = 0.896
Upward social comparison on
USC2 0.802 ***
USC3 0.715 ***
USC4 0.755 ***
USC5 0.777 ***
USC6 0.861 ***
EV2 0.810 ***
EV3 0.779 ***
EV4 0.797 ***
EV5 0.851 ***
IB2 0.807 ***
IB3 0.788 ***
IB4 0.756 ***
IB5 0.758 ***
IB6 0.763 ***
IB7 0.841 ***
IB8 0.851 ***
SE2 0.832 ***
SE3 0.783 ***
SE4 0.746 ***
SE5 0.779 ***
SE6 0.777 ***
SE7 0.753 ***
SE8 0.728 ***
SE9 0.792 ***
SE10 0.851 ***
Table 3 The correlation matrix and discriminant validity
N = 318 Average variances extracted (AVEs) are in bold
The correlation coefficients are in italics
Constructs USC EV IB SE
Trang 9(with bootstrap resamples N = 5000) Table 5 shows that the
95% bias-corrected confidence interval for the indirect effect
of benign envy on impulse buying is between 0.144 and
0.317, which excludes the value of zero The mediating path
is also significant (β = 0.226; p < 0.001) The results confirm
that benign envy mediates the effect of upward social
com-parison on SCPs on impulse buying
Moderating Effect of Self‑esteem
To examine moderating role of self-esteem, we used model 59
of PROCESS macro (Hayes, 2013; Hayes et al., 2017) First, the
direct effect of self-esteem on impulse buying was significant
(p < 0.01) Next, we examined whether interaction term
(self-esteem x upward social comparison) has a significant effect
on impulse buying The result shows a significant interaction
effect (β = −0.09, p = 0.024 < 0.05), indicating that self-esteem
negatively moderates the relationship between upward social
comparison on SCPs and impulse buying Thus, the H4a is
sup-ported In addition, the effect of interaction term (self-esteem
x benign envy) on impulse buying is also significant (β = 0.12,
p = 0.006 < 0.01) This result suggests that self-esteem moderates
the relationship benign envy and impulse buying, but this
mod-erating effect is positive Thus, the H4b is partially supported
self-esteem reports a higher level of impulse buying when making comparisons on SCPs It means that the positive effect of upward social comparison on SCPs on impulse buy-ing is more pronounced when self-esteem is low rather than when it is high However, the positive effect of benign envy
on impulse buying is strengthened for consumers with high level of self-esteem (see Fig. 4)
Discussion
This study set out to examine the effect of upward social comparison on impulse buying in the context of image-shar-ing SCPs through a moderated mediation model, in which benign envy is a mediator and self-esteem is a moderator Overall, the empirical results lend support to all proposed hypotheses In the following, the key findings of this study are discussed in more detail
First, the results confirm a significant and positive effect of upward social comparison on benign envy The result deepens our understanding of how benign envy was elicited by upward social comparisons occurring on image-sharing SCPs This finding is consistent with extant literature (e.g., Latif et al., 2021; Appel et al., 2016) which indicates that social compari-son on Facebook elicits benign envy Additionally, the finding is
Table 4 Path coefficients and significances
USC = Upward social comparison on SCPs,
EV = Benign envy, IB = Impulse buying, *** p < 0.001
Hypothesis Path Standardized path
coefficient Result H1 USC ➔ EV 0.727 *** Support
H2 EV ➔ IB 0.532 *** Support
H3 USC ➔ IB 0.386 *** Support
Fig 2 The path coefficients of
research model USC = Upward
social comparison on SCPs, BE
= Benign envy; SE =
Self-esteem *** p < 0.001; ** p <
0.01; * p < 0.05
Table 5 Mediating effect testing result
USC = Upward social comparison on SCPs, EV = Benign envy,
IB = Impulse buying, *: p < 0.001
Path Indirect effect 95% Confidence
interval Lower Upper USC ➔ EV ➔ IB 0.226 *** 0.144 0.317
Trang 10also consistent with the study of Li (2018) that adolescents are
still developing their own self-concept, which is conducive to
self-evaluating and self-improving Nevertheless, due to a lack
of objective standards for self-evaluation, they tend to take the
better people as a yardstick to evaluate themselves, thus
eas-ily exposing themselves to envy Furthermore, image-sharing
SCPs users are more likely to be visually appealed by product
images featured by online celebrities or influencers who have a
vast number of media followers (Djafarova & Rushworth, 2017;
Pittman & Reich, 2016) As a result, image-sharing SCP users
are prone to make upward social comparisons with these digital celebrities, prompting benign envy
Second, upward social comparison on SCPs affects impulse buying directly and indirectly On the one hand, the direct effect of upward social comparison on impulse buying suggests that users are susceptible to impulse purchases after upwardly comparing with digital celebrities on Instagram or Pinterest whose captivating pictures are advertised for specific products
It also consolidates the findings from prior studies in various contexts, such as social networking sites (Liu et al., 2019) and
Fig 3 Plotted interaction of
upward social comparison
on SCPs and self-esteem on
impulse buying
Fig 4 Plotted interaction of
benign envy and self-esteem on
impulse buying