1. Trang chủ
  2. » Giáo Dục - Đào Tạo

Sensory perception during sleep in human

15 0 0

Đang tải... (xem toàn văn)

Tài liệu hạn chế xem trước, để xem đầy đủ mời bạn chọn Tải xuống

THÔNG TIN TÀI LIỆU

Thông tin cơ bản

Định dạng
Số trang 15
Dung lượng 884,09 KB

Nội dung

Sleep Medicine (2003) 493–507 www.elsevier.com/locate/sleep Review Sensory perception during sleep in humans: a magnetoencephalograhic study Ryusuke Kakigia,b,*, Daisuke Nakac, Tomohiro Okusaa, Xiohong Wanga,b, Koji Inuia, Yunhai Qiua,b, Tuan Diep Trana, Kensaku Mikia,b, Yohei Tamuraa,b, Thi Binh Nguyena,b, Shoko Watanabea, Minoru Hoshiyamaa,d a b Department of Integrative Physiology, National Institute for Physiological Sciences, Myodaiji, Okazaki 444-8585, Japan Department of Physiological Sciences, School of Life Sciences, The Graduate University for Advanced Studies, Okazaki 444-8585, Japan c Department of Neurological Surgery, Wakayama Medical University, Wakayama 641-8510, Japan d Department of Health Sciences, Faculty of Medicine, Nagoya University, Nagoya 461-8673, Japan Received 19 March 2003; received in revised form 12 May 2003; accepted 14 May 2003 Abstract We reported the changes of brain responses during sleep following auditory, visual, somatosensory and painful somatosensory stimulation by using magnetoencephalography (MEG) Surprisingly, very large changes were found under all conditions, although the changes in each were not the same However, there are some common findings Short-latency components, reflecting the primary cortical activities generated in the primary sensory cortex for each stimulus kind, show no significant change, or are slightly prolonged in latency and decreased in amplitude These findings indicate that the neuronal activities in the primary sensory cortex are not affected or are only slightly inhibited during sleep By contrast, middle- and long-latency components, probably reflecting secondary activities, are much affected during sleep Since the dipole location is changed (auditory stimulation), unchanged (somatosensory stimulation) or vague (visual stimulation) between the state of being awake and asleep, different regions responsible for such changes of activity may be one explanation, although the activated regions are very close to each other The enhancement of activities probably indicates two possibilities, an increase in the activity of excitatory systems during sleep, or a decrease in the activity of some inhibitory systems, which are active in the awake state We have no evidence to support either, but we prefer the latter, since it is difficult to consider why neuronal activities would be increased during sleep q 2003 Elsevier B.V All rights reserved Keywords: Magnetoencephalography; Sleep; Auditory; Visual; Somatosensory; Pain; Electroencephalography Introduction One is, of course, not aware of auditory, visual, somatosensory and pain stimuli while asleep, but such signals should reach the brain nonetheless It is important to know what happens in the brain when stimulation is given during sleep and how brain responses following auditory, visual and somatosensory stimulation differ from the waking state to that of sleep To investigate the brain responses to a stimulus, several non-invasive methods are used Recent advances in neuroimaging now enable such responses to be monitored, even during sleep To examine * Corresponding author Address: Department of Integrative Physiology National Institute for Physiological Sciences, Myodaiji, Okazaki 444-8585, Japan Tel.: ỵ 81-564-55-7765x7779; fax: ỵ 81-564-52-7913 E-mail address: kakigi@nips.ac.jp (R Kakigi) 1389-9457/$ - see front matter q 2003 Elsevier B.V All rights reserved doi:10.1016/S1389-9457(03)00169-2 the changes in cortical activity during the early processing of stimulus perception, within 100 or 200 ms after stimulus onset, electroencephalography (EEG) and magnetoencephalography (MEG) are the most appropriate methods; their temporal resolution is very high, in the order of milliseconds, as compared with other neuroimaging techniques such as positron emission tomography (PET) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) whose temporal resolution is over 1000 ms MEG has theoretical advantages over EEG for detecting source localization because there is less effect from current conductivity caused by cerebrospinal fluid, skull and skin; the spatial resolution for MEG is only a few millimeters, but that for EEG is much larger Therefore, we studied the effects of sleep on brain responses by analyzing evoked magnetic fields following auditory (AEF) [1], visual (VEF) [2], 494 R Kakigi et al / Sleep Medicine (2003) 493–507 somatosensory (SEF) [3] and painful somatosensory (painrelated SEF) [4,5] stimulation The objective of this article is to review our results, most of which are the first to report the effects of sleep on evoked magnetic fields following each kind of sensory stimulation Comparison of MEG with EEG results (auditory, visual, somatosensory, and painful somatosensory evoked potentials, AEP, VEP, SEP and painrelated SEP, respectively) was often very useful landmarks used to create the head-based three-dimensional (3D) coordinate system (the nasion and bilateral preauricular points) were visualized in the MR images by affixing to these points high-contrast cod liver oil capsules (3 mm diameter) The common MEG and MRI anatomical landmarks allowed easy transformation of the head-based 3D coordinate system (nasion and entrance of the auditory meatus of the left and right ear) used for the MEG source analysis to the MRI MEG system Issues common to all studies Unlike EEG, MEG is not well known The magnetic fields recorded from the human brain are very small, approximately ten thousand to a million times smaller than the earth’s steady magnetic field and environmental fields (caused by a train, for example) A superconducting quantum interference device (SQUID) is necessary to detect these weak fields We use dual 37-channel axial-type firstorder biomagnetometers (Magnes, Biomagnetic Technologies Inc (BTi), San Diego, CA) The waveforms from 74 channels are thus simultaneously recorded The detection coils of the biomagnetometers are arranged in a uniformly distributed array in concentric circles over a spherically concave surface All of the sensor coils are thus equally sensitive to the brain’s weak magnetic signals Each device is 144 mm in diameter with a radius of 122 mm The outer coils are 72.58 apart, and each coil is connected to a SQUID The spacing between the centers of the coils is 22 mm The coils are 20 mm in diameter and have a 50 mm baseline Recordings were performed in a magnetically shielded room (MSR, Vacuumschmelze GmbH) The placement of probes for our MEG was based on the international 10/20 EEG system Dual probes centered at positions C3 and C4 were used to cover the left and right hemisphere for recording AEF and SEF, and at position Oz to cover the occipital region for recording VEF A spherical model [6] was fitted to the digitized shape of the head of each subject, and the location (x; y and z location), orientation and amplitude of the best-fitted equivalent current dipole(s) (ECD) were estimated at each time point The origin of the head-based coordinate system was the midpoint between the preauricular points The x-axis indicated the coronal plane with a positive value toward the left preauricular point, and the z-axis lay on the transverse plane perpendicular to the x – y line with a positive value toward the upper side The correlation between the recorded measurements and the values expected from the ECD estimate was calculated as a measure of how closely the measured values corresponded to the theoretical field generated by the model and the observed field Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) was performed using a Magnex 150XT 1.5T system (Shimadzu, Kyoto, Japan) T1-weighted coronal, axial and sagittal images with a contiguous 1.5 mm slice thickness were used for overlays with ECD sources detected by MEG The same anatomical All 22 subjects who participated in this study were researchers and technicians in our department (six females and 16 males; mean age 29.7 years, range 27 –45 years), who were very familiar with MEG studies None of the subjects had any history of otological, ophthalmological, or neurological abnormalities or sleep disorders Some number of the 22 subjects participated in each study All subjects were right-handed Informed consent was obtained from all participants prior to the study, which was first approved by the Ethical Committee at our institute Experiments were performed in the daytime, usually after lunch No medication was given to the subjects before the experiment A somnogram was obtained for all subjects by recording EEG with the use of gold disk electrodes (1 cm diameter) which were placed at the Cz, Pz and Oz positions Three channels were used for the bipolar recording of EEG between Cz –Pz, Cz – Oz and Pz– Oz The sleep stage was determined according to the guidelines of Rechtschaffen and Kales [7] using the continuously recorded somnogram data We carefully observed the subject’s movements through a TV monitor during the experiments, and confirmed the position of the head using the head location sensor When a movement was large, we discarded the data Since most subjects did not reach stage 3, stage or rapid eye movement (REM) sleep and body movements during stage and stage were sometimes very large, we analyzed results in the awake state (AW), stage sleep and stage sleep in each study All experiments were done at least twice to confirm the reproducibility of results For obtaining clear responses, 100 –200 responses were averaged Trials in which the MEG deflection exceeded pT were excluded from the averaging We measured the latency, amplitude and ECD location for each component The amplitude of MEG was calculated using the maximum amplitude of the outgoing and ingoing magnetic fields of each component The latency and ECD location were measured at the time of maximum amplitude in each component To determine the difference in effect between AW and the sleep stages (stages and 2) more clearly, we assigned a value of 100% to the amplitudes of the waveforms in AW in some studies For example, if the amplitudes of the waveforms in AW and stage sleep were 100 and 80 fT, respectively, the amplitude ratio was 80% R Kakigi et al / Sleep Medicine (2003) 493–507 Two criteria were used in the application of the ECD model to a magnetic response First, during the period of the response, the location of the dipole determined by the ECD model must remain stationary (within mm of each coordinate) Second, the correlation between the recorded magnetic fields and values expected from the dipole estimate must have a value larger than 0.95 495 The differences of latency, amplitude (or amplitude ratio) and ECD locations among AW and the sleep stages (stages and 2) were statistically analyzed using a one-way factorial analysis of variance (ANOVA) with Bonferroni – Dunn’s correction or Fisher’s protected least significant difference (PLSD) test for multiple comparison Significance was accepted at the 0.05 level in all statistical analyses Fig AEF during the awake state and sleep stages and A pure tone of 250 Hz was delivered to the right ear and the magnetometer was placed on the left hemisphere (position C3) Thirty-seven superimposed waveforms at each stage for subject are shown Four main components (M50, M100, M150 and M200) were identified in each stage Although M50 and M100 showed no definite change during the two stages of sleep, M150 and M200 were significantly enhanced in both sleep stages ðp , 0:05Þ: Peak latency of M150 and M200 to 250 Hz tone during sleep did not show a significant change as shown in Table 1, and they appeared shortened during sleep in this subject However, M50, M100 and M200 were significantly prolonged to 1000 or 4000 Hz (Table 1) Adopted from Ref [1] 496 R Kakigi et al / Sleep Medicine (2003) 493–507 Auditory evoked magnetic field (AEF) 4.1 Methods Ten normal volunteers were studied A pure tone of 70 dB sound pressure level (SPL) 70 ms in duration (including rise and fall time, 10 ms each) was delivered to the right ear at a rate of Hz through a soft plastic tube Three different frequencies of pure tone, i.e 250, 1000 and 4000 Hz, were presented in the awake state and in each sleep stage (sleep stages and 2) White noise of 50 dB SPL was delivered to the opposite (left) ear through the same soft plastic tube during the recordings The recordings were performed during naturally occurring daytime sleep in a magnetically shielded room with the subject lying on a bed Responses were filtered with a 0.1– 200 Hz bandpass filter and digitized at a sampling rate of 1041.7 Hz The analysis time was 100 ms before and 1000 ms after the application of the pure tone stimuli We set the amplitude ratio of each component in AW as 100% 4.2 Results We focused on the results recorded from the left hemisphere, contralateral to the stimulated ear Four components were identified with latencies of less than 250 ms at each frequency tone (Fig 1) The latency of each component in AW, S1 and S2 following 250, 1000 and 4000 Hz tone stimuli was approximately 50– 60, 95 – 110, 150 –165 and 200 –215 ms, and we termed them M50, M100, M150 and M200, respectively The sleep stage affected the waveforms of each of the four components Therefore, to avoid confusion, we use the following abbreviations to show each component in each condition: AW for awake, S1 for stage and S2 for stage 2, for example, ‘M50-1000 Hz-AW’ means the M50 component of AEF following the 1000 Hz pure tone stimulation in the awake state M50-1000 Hz-S2 ðP , 0:05Þ; M100-1000 Hz-S2 ðP , 0:01Þ; M200-4000 Hz-S1 ðP , 0:05Þ and M200-4000 Hz-S2 ðP , 0:05Þ showed a significant prolongation from those components in AW (Table 1) Some other components showed a similar tendency, but the changes were not significant, probably due to a large inter-individual difference For example, M150 and M200 to 250 Hz in subject appeared shortened during sleep (Fig 1) To more clearly identify the effects of sleep on amplitude, we used the amplitude ratio (Table 1) In general, with an increase of sleep stage, there was a tendency for amplitude reduction in the early-latency components (M50 and M100) and amplitude enhancement in the late-latency components (M150 and M200) M150250 Hz-S1, M200-250 Hz-S1, M150-250 Hz-S2, M200250 Hz-S2, M200-1000 Hz-S2, M200-4000 Hz-S1, M150-4000 Hz-S2 and M200-4000 Hz-S2 showed significant enhancement compared with the same components in AW (Table 1, Fig 2) Although M150-1000 Hz-S1, Table Peak latencies, amplitudes and ECD locations (x; y; z values) of identifiable components in the left hemisphere following a pure tone stimulus of three different frequencies (250, 1000 and 4000 Hz) applied to the right ear in the awake state (AW) and sleep stages (S1) and (S2) Peak latency (ms) M50-1000 Hz-AW M50-1000 Hz-S2 M100-1000 Hz-AW M100-1000 Hz-S2 M200-4000 Hz-AW M200-4000 Hz-S1 M200-4000 Hz-S2 49.4 (8.1) 58.5 (5.8)* 94.7 (7.1) 104.9 (9.8)** 197.2 (13.0) 208.4 (9.4)* 206.6 (11.0)* Peak amplitude ratio (%) based on results in AW as 100% M150-250 Hz-S1 149.1 (67.7)* M200-250 Hz-S1 125.7 (35.7)* M150-250 Hz-S2 158.1 (59.1)* M200-250 Hz-S2 153.8 (69.6)* M200-4000 Hz-S1 128.6 (30.2)* M150-4000 Hz-S2 176.5 (63.3)** M200-4000 Hz-S2 174.3 (62.9)** ECD location (cm): z-axis M100-1000 Hz-AW M100-1000 Hz-S1 M100-1000 Hz-S2 5.35 (0.97) 5.76 (0.87)* 6.55 (0.69)** Only the results that showed a significant difference are given Values are shown as the mean (standard deviation) Statistical analysis was done using an one-way factorial analysis of variance (ANOVA) with Bonferroni – Dunn’s correction for multiple comparison *P , 0:05; **P , 0:01; compared with AW M200-1000 Hz-S1, M150-1000 Hz-S2 and M1504000 Hz-S1 showed a tendency for enhancement, their changes were not significant The ECD location of every component was estimated for each stimulus frequency and each sleep stage Every ECD in all conditions was estimated to be around the superior temporal cortex (the primary and secondary auditory cortex) (Fig 3) Concerning the effect of sleep stage on ECD location, in the early-latency components (M50 and M100), the M50 component had a tendency to be located in a more anterior, lateral and superior region in the left primary auditory cortex when the sleep stage was deeper As for the M100 component, the ECDs tended to be located in a more anterior, medial and superior region in accord with the depth of sleep However, the only components to show a significant change were M100-1000 Hz-S1 ðP , 0:05Þ and M100-1000 Hz-S2 ðP , 0:01Þ; which were more superior along the z-axis than M100-1000 Hz-AW (Table 1) In the late-latency components (M150 and M200), there was no consistent tendency for change in ECD location between each sleep stage Concerning the effect of stimulus frequency on ECD location, the y coordinate (medio-lateral direction) showed smaller values when the delivered frequency was increased, that is, the ECDs of high-frequency tone stimuli were located more medially than those of low-frequency tone stimuli in all sleep stages (Fig 3) R Kakigi et al / Sleep Medicine (2003) 493–507 497 Fig Changes of amplitude ratio (% index) of each component recorded from the left hemisphere in response to a 250 Hz tone delivered to the right ear in both sleep stages (sleep stages and 2) compared with a corresponding component in the awake state (100%) The statistical analysis comparing the awake state with both sleep stages was performed with a one-way factorial analysis of variance (ANOVA) with Bonferroni –Dunn’s correction for multiple comparison The M150 and M200 components of both stages of sleep were significantly enhanced compared with those of the awake state (probability, *P , 0:05) Adopted from Ref [1] 4.3 Discussion This is, to our knowledge, the first report of AEF changes during sleep indicating a physiological change of auditory perception The most important and interesting findings of the present study are the changes of waveforms and ECD location of each component during sleep There was a reduction in the amplitude of M50 and M100 and an enhancement of M150 and M200 In a study of cats, Chen and Buchwald [8] reported a ‘wave A’ recorded from the vertex 20– 22 ms in latency It decreased in amplitude or disappeared during slow wave sleep (SWS), but dramatically increased in amplitude or reappeared during the REM stage The amplitude during the REM stage was equal to that while awake This positive potential in the cat showed the same characteristics as M50 in the present study Furthermore, Steriade et al [9] described that the discharge rates of the ascending reticular formation neurons in cats were increased during the awake state and REM sleep, but decreased during SWS From the results of these animal experiments and the AEF amplitude changes in the present study, we speculate that the amplitude reduction of the early-latency components (M50 and M100) during sleep is caused by a decrease in the sensitivity of the primary auditory processing system which may be controlled by the ascending reticular formation As for the long-latency components, Wasensten and Badia [10] reported that the amplitude and latency of N200, probably corresponding to M200 in the present study, 498 R Kakigi et al / Sleep Medicine (2003) 493–507 Fig Location of ECDs of the M100 component observed in response to three different frequencies (250, 1000 and 4000 Hz) in subject The magnetometer was placed on the left hemisphere (C3 position), and ECD overlapped on MRI (coronal slices) in each sleep stage All ECDs were located in the superior temporal cortex (the auditory cortex) However, ECDs were located deeper in response to the higher frequency tones than lower frequency tones Adopted from Ref [1] increased during sleep, and the amplitude was highest in sleep stages and These observations suggest that the generator systems may be different from those of the early-latency components functionally and anatomically The enhancement of amplitude for the M150 and M200 components during sleep in the present study might be due to a decrease of inhibitory activity of the secondary auditory processing system, for example the cognitive processing system at the cortical level The location of the ECD of each component moved in the medial direction with an increase of tone frequency during both the awake state and sleep This result was compatible with those in previous studies of the spatial tonotopic organization in monkeys [11] and humans [12] during the awake state The ECD location of the earlylatency components, M50 and M100, were located at more anterior and superior sites during sleep than AW In contrast, the ECD location of the late-latency components, M150 and M200, during sleep did not show such consistent differences compared with that during AW Therefore, the mechanisms generating the late-latency components may be different from those behind the early-latency components the experiment Responses were filtered with a 0.1 – 200 Hz bandpass filter and digitized at a sampling rate of 1041.7 Hz The analysis time was 100 ms before and 300 ms after the application of the flash stimuli The flash stimulus was delivered with a light stimulator (SLS4100, Nihonkohden, Japan) every s throughout the session The light was placed at a distance of 2.5 m from the eyes of the subjects The direction of light was about 408 below the line of fixation of the subjects While the subjects slept for 40– 60 min, – 11 trials were recorded for various sleep stages After the subjects awoke, the awake VEF were recorded During the recording of the awake VEF, the subjects were required to keep their eyes closed To examine the similarity between the magnetic components of awake and asleep, contour maps of the isomagnetic field were used The contour maps show a topographical distribution of the magnetic flux density on the surface of the scalp We calculated the correlation coefficient across 37 channels between contour maps of sleep and awake VEF, and judged the similarity to be high when the correlation value was greater than 0.52 (P ¼ 0:001; degree of freedom ¼ 35) Even if the peak latency and amplitude differed, similar contour maps indicate common generating mechanisms Visual evoked magnetic field(s) (VEF) 5.2 Results 5.1 Methods Fig shows the VEF waveforms for AW, sleep stage and sleep stage in four subjects, showing an interindividual difference Fig shows the nomenclature of each identifiable component and their isocontour maps in two subjects The waveforms for the awake condition showed multiple peaks from 40 to 200 ms, that is, A1 (41.4 ^ 4.5 ms, mean ^ standard deviation (SD)), A2 (54.2 ^ 4.3 ms), A3 (63.5 ^ 5.0 ms), A4 (76.2 ^ 6.4 ms), A5 (93.7 ^ 8.2 ms), A6 (112.1 ^ 6.5 ms), A7 (140.0 ^ 8.8 ms) and A8 (186.0 ^ 9.6 ms) A1 to A3 composed the first deflection of the VEF waveform, and Nine normal subjects were studied The center of the array was set around the Oz position covering the visual cortex of the subjects The head was fixed to the biomagnetometer with adhesive tape to prevent movement The subjects wore a bandage on their right eye and were instructed to close their eyes throughout the experiment, even when awake To mask the noise caused by the strobe light, rubber earplugs were provided and white noise of 50 dB was delivered from a loud speaker during R Kakigi et al / Sleep Medicine (2003) 493–507 499 Fig VEF waveforms for the awake state, sleep stage 1, and sleep stage The data from two subjects are shown A flash was delivered at ms on the horizontal axis A sharp large deflection at around ms due to stimulus artifacts was excluded from the figure The inter-individual difference was large in the awake VEF, but the sleep VEF was rather similar among subjects, with one large peak at about 80–100 ms The VEFs were similar between stages and 2, though the peak amplitude and latency increased for stage Adopted from Ref [2] A4 to A6 the second A7 and A8 were late and slow deflections These components were considered to generally correspond to the flash visual evoked potential (VEP) components reported by Ciganek [13] In contrast, the waveforms during sleep had fewer peaks, which were late in latency and large in amplitude We identified three peaks, S1 (64.2 ^ 3.5 ms in stage and 65.5 ^ 3.5 ms in stage 2), S2 (90.2 ^ 7.3 ms in stage and 100.5 ^ 20.3 ms in stage 2) and S3 (113.7 ^ 12.8 ms in stage and 115.5 ^ 14.5 ms in stage 2) in the sleep VEF from the root mean square (RMS) time course S1 was not identified in some subjects (i.e subject 2) S2 overlapped S3, and in some subjects S2 seemed a notch peak on the larger S3 component The longer latency components (later than 130 ms) for the sleep conditions were much weaker than S2 and S3, and not clearly identifiable Because waveforms of the awake and sleep VEF differed markedly (e.g subject 2), we used maps of the isomagnetic field (contour map) to find correspondence between the eight components for the awake VEF and the three for the sleep VEF The correlation coefficient across 37 channels between each pair of contour maps was used to measure the similarity between the maps Fig shows the contour maps at each peak of A1 –A8 and S1 –S3 in two subjects Based on the correlation coefficiency, A2, A5 and A6 were considered to correspond to S1, S2 and S3, respectively Other components in the awake VEF were not significantly correlated with the components in the sleep VEF After matching the components, we compared their peak latency and peak amplitude values The latencies of S1, S2 and S3 increased for A2, A5 and A6, respectively The difference was significant between S1 and A2, and between S3 and A6 (P , 0:05; by the paired t-test with Bonferroni – Dunn’s correction) The latencies were longer in stage than stage 1, though not significantly The amplitudes of these components also showed a tendency to increase with sleep stage The difference was significant between A5 and S2, and between A6 and S3 ðP , 0:05Þ: 500 R Kakigi et al / Sleep Medicine (2003) 493–507 Fig Contour maps for identified components of the awake and sleep VEF are shown in two subjects (subjects and 2) Vertical lines superimposed on the waveforms indicate the latencies at which the contour maps were drawn In the sleep VEF, not only maps for S1 to S3, but also those for 45, 75 ms and so on were drawn for comparison In each contour map, areas of the same magnetic flux density are drawn in the same color (see the inset) Red and pink indicate the flux going into the head, while blue indicates that coming out from the head The corresponding components are indicated with arrows, with the correlation coefficiency value between the contour maps Adopted from Ref [2] The signal source for the VEF was estimated using an equivalent current dipole (ECD) model The ECDs were located in the primary visual cortex (V1) for the early components in the awake VEF (A1 – A6), and three components in the sleep VEF (S1 – S3), but the estimation was unsuccessful for the later components This was probably due to the complicated activities generated in multiple regions in the visual cortex 5.3 Discussion To our knowledge, this is the first study on the changes in VEF during sleep, although there have been several studies on the changes in VEP during sleep in neonates In fullterm neonates, a positive component with a latency of about 200 ms was clearly identified while the subjects were awake, but it disappeared during sleep [14] In preterm neonates, a negative component at about 300 ms was prominent in the awake state, but its amplitude was markedly reduced during sleep [15] These studies suggest that the change in VEF due to sleep occurs in the early stages of development However, the VEP waveforms in the neonates were so different from those in adults that it is difficult to compare them with the present results In the present study, the early component (A2) showed a slight prolongation of latency, and the middle latency components (A5 and A6) showed an enhancement of amplitude As in the AEF study [1], the decrease in the amplitude of the early components might be due to a decrease in the sensitivity of the primary processing system, and on the other hand, the increase in the late components might be due to the decrease in inhibitory activity Although some components of the awake VEF were identified in the sleep VEF, others were not Thus several components (e.g A3, A7 or A8) of the awake VEF were considered to have disappeared, or to overlap with the other components, with the latency shift during sleep The reason for the change is not clear, but both the change in the neuronal activity of the lateral geniculate nuclei (LGN) and the change in the activity of the cortex are considered to cause the VEF change Neuronal recordings in animals have revealed that the mode of activity of neurons in the LGN differed between those asleep and those awake [16 –18] The change in the relaying neurons is thought to affect the VEF during sleep The multiple components of the awake VEF are considered to be partially due to the construction of the visual cortex, which is composed of a network of more than 30 sub-cortical areas [19] V1 is reported to be the main signal source of VEP and VEF [20,21] and receives visual information not only via the LGN but also from the higher extrastriate cortical areas [19] The disappearance of the late components (A7 and A8) during sleep might reflect the change in the activity of the higher area Lamme et al [21] reported that when a monkey was anesthetized the modulatory activity of the V1 neurons (later than 100 ms) decreased, which might be related to feedback from the extrastriate cortex The activity pattern is considered to change during sleep, resulting in the change in the VEF R Kakigi et al / Sleep Medicine (2003) 493–507 Somatosensory evoked magnetic fields following non-painful (SEF) and painful stimulation (pain-related SEF) 6.1 Methods Eight normal subjects were studied The electrical stimulus was a constant current square wave pulse delivered transcutaneously to the left index finger using ring electrodes The stimulus duration was 1.0 ms Two levels of intensity were adopted, ‘painful’ and ‘non-painful’ The degree of pain was about 80% of intolerable pain, depending on the subjective feeling of each subject, and its intensity ranged from 14 to 16 mA The level of non-painful stimulation was three times the sensory threshold, approximately 3–4 mA The inter-stimulus interval was random, between 1500 and 5000 ms Basic methods were based on previous studies [23–26] Two measurement matrices were centered around the C3 and C4 of the international 10–20 system in each subject The primary and secondary somatosensory cortices (SI and SII, respectively) in the bilateral hemispheres were mostly covered by these positions SEF responses were filtered with a 0.1 –100 Hz bandpass filter and digitized at a sampling rate of 1042 Hz The analysis time was 100 ms before and 500 ms after the application of each stimulus A single dipole spherical model was estimated for each point in time [6] Since the measured field in several subjects was considered to contain two or more temporally overlapping sources (as will be described in the results) the single ECD model was inappropriate in such cases Therefore, for the spatio-temporal multi-dipole model, we used brain electric source analysis (BESA 99, NeuroScan, Inc., McLean, VA) for the computation of theoretical source generators in a three-layer spherical head model BESA was modified for our 37-channel biomagnetometer [27 – 29] The residual variance (%RV) indicated the percentage of data that could not be explained by the model The goodness-offit (GOF) was expressed as a percentage (100 %RV) The GOF indicated the percentage of the data that can be explained by the model, which is different from the correlation value calculated in a single ECD model We considered the adaptation of the dipoles to be significant when the GOF was larger than 90% 6.2 Results During the awake state, five consistent components were identified from the hemisphere contralateral to the stimulated finger in both non-painful and painful sessions We termed them 1M, 2M, 3M, 4M, and 5M (Figs and 7) The peak latency and amplitude of each component are shown in Table The peak latency of each component did not differ between the two stimulus conditions Although 1M, 2M and 3M, which peaked around 20, 35 and 45 ms, respectively, did not differ in amplitude between painful 501 and non-painful stimulation, the amplitudes of 4M and 5M, which peaked around 80 and 160 ms, respectively, were significantly larger in the painful sessions ðP , 0:02Þ: Using a single dipole model, ECDs for 1M, 2M and 3M were located in the posterior bank of the central sulcus, probably the SI, following both non-painful and painful stimulation The isocontour map of 4M was much different from the maps of 1M – 3M, indicating that different mechanisms are involved The ECD for 4M was localized in the upper bank of the Sylvian fissure, probably in the SII, following both non-painful and painful stimulation (Figs and 7) Since isocontour maps of 5M showed a complicated dipole pattern in both non-painful and painful sessions and the ECD could not be reliably estimated by a single dipole model, 5M was considered to contain two or more sources In the hemisphere ipsilateral to the stimulation, two components, 4M(I) and 5M(I) whose latencies were longer than those of 4M and 5M by approximately 10 –15 ms, were identified in both stimulus conditions (Fig 7) Amplitudes of these components were significantly larger for painful stimulation ðP , 0:02Þ (Table 2) The correlation value for 4M(I) was over 97% following both painful and non-painful stimulation The ECD for 4M(I) was localized to SII The correlation value for 5M(I) was relatively low following both painful and non-painful stimulation, but the ECD of 5M(I) was estimated in SII in all subjects During sleep, the amplitude of 1M and 2M was slightly increased and decreased, respectively, but these changes did not reach the significant level ðP 0:05Þ: By contrast, the amplitude of 3M ðP , 0:05Þ; 4M ðP , 0:005Þ and 5M ðP , 0:005Þ were significantly decreased during sleep (Table 2) The two components detected in the ipsilateral hemisphere, 4M(I) and 5M(I), were significantly decreased in amplitude ðP , 0:005Þ during the sleep stages in a similar manner to 4M and 5M (Table 2) For the analysis using the multi-dipole model, BESA, we found that a two-dipole model placing source and at SI and SII, respectively, was the most appropriate In the time range up to 50 ms, it was considered that only S1 was activated from the results of the single dipole model, and the period from the onset of the 4M component to the offset of the 5M component was used as the analysis period for BESA The GOF value was not reliably high (65.5 ^ 25.6%) with the SII source alone, but it markedly and reliably increased when the SI source was added (91.4 ^ 1.0%) We could not make a better model than this one Two components were identified in the SII source, an early and late component, and the source orientation of the former and latter was reversed The strength of both the early and late components of the SII source was significantly decreased during sleep ðP , 0:001Þ (Table 3, Fig 8) The source strength of both the early and late components was reduced in stage sleep compared to stage sleep, but the difference was not significant Peak latencies of early and late components were prolonged along with 502 R Kakigi et al / Sleep Medicine (2003) 493–507 Fig SEF following painful and non-painful electrical stimulation of the index finger in the awake state, and stage and sleep Waveforms recorded from 37 channels in the hemisphere contralateral to the stimulation are superimposed During sleep, 1M and 2M showed no significant change, 3M was reduced in amplitude, and 4M and 5M were much reduced Isocontour maps of 2M and 4M at the peak latency following painful stimulation while awake Thin lines show fields directed out of the head, dotted lines those into the head, and thick lines the zero fields Isocontour lines are separated by 20 fT The ECD of 2M and 4M was estimated to lie in the SI and SII, respectively Adopted from Ref [4] R Kakigi et al / Sleep Medicine (2003) 493–507 503 Fig SEF following painful electrical stimulation of the index finger recorded from the hemisphere contralateral and ipsilateral to the stimulation while awake and in stage sleep 4M(I) and 5M(I) recorded from the hemisphere ipsilateral to the stimulation were much reduced in amplitude during sleep, like 4M and 5M in the contralateral hemisphere Isocontour maps of 4M and 4M(I) at the peak latency following painful stimulation while awake The ECD of 4M and 4M(I) was estimated to lie in the SII of each hemisphere Adopted from Ref [4] 504 R Kakigi et al / Sleep Medicine (2003) 493–507 Table Peak latencies and amplitudes of SEF components Non-painful, awake 1M 2M 3M 4M 5M 4M(I) 5M(I) (fT) (ms) (fT) (ms) (fT) (ms) (fT) (ms) (fT) (ms) (fT) (ms) (fT) (ms) 31.1 ^ 12.2 19.6 ^ 0.5 42.2 ^ 20.4 33.7 ^ 5.4 44.1 ^ 21.2 43.3 ^ 2.3 100.3 ^ 47.5{{ 82.0 ^ 9.9 78.2 ^ 33.8{ 154.9 ^ 22.3 65.1 ^ 30.6{{ 92.5 ^ 6.4 57.0 ^ 19.1{ 170.2 ^ 14.2 Painful Awake Stage Stage 36.4 ^ 15.9 19.3 ^ 1.3 55.6 ^ 24.2 34.0 ^ 5.0 61.9 ^ 24.3 43.7 ^ 5.6 153.3 ^ 62.8 86.3 ^ 7.6 119.1 ^ 32.8 156.2 ^ 22.0 103.3 ^ 41.6 95.8 ^ 5.7 92.0 ^ 28.8 172.6 ^ 13.3 41.7 ^ 18.5 19.6 ^ 0.9 47.5 ^ 19.2 34.2 ^ 5.2 36.1 ^ 15.3* 44.0 ^ 2.8 76.4 ^ 32.4** – 69.7 ^ 28.5** – 49.8 ^ 20.7** – 46.4 ^ 14.2** – 39.6 ^ 6.1 20.3 ^ 1.8 52.3 ^ 20.0 35.4 ^ 5.6 32.2 ^ 14.7* 44.3 ^ 3.1 69.7 ^ 30.0** – 51.9 ^ 15.4** – 36.4 ^ 12.0** – 41.1 ^ 21.6** – { P , 0:02; {{P , 0:01 compared with the results following painful stimulation while awake (paired t-test) *P , 0:05; **P , 0:005 compared with the results following painful stimulation in the awake state (ANOVA followed by post-hoc paired comparisons using Fisher’s PLSD procedure) Peak latencies of 4M, 5M, 4M(I) and 5M(I) during sleep are not shown in this table, since they could not be accurately determined in most subjects the stage of sleep, but the prolongation was not significant ðP 0:05Þ: 6.3 Discussion The advantage of this study is that signals ascending through both fast A-b fibers relating to touch and slow A-d fibers relating to pain can be recorded simultaneously Since the conduction velocity of A-b fibers and A-d fibers following the electrical stimulation of skin is approximately 50 –60 and 10 –15 m/s [30], signals ascending through A-b and A-d fibers are considered to reach the cerebral cortex approximately 20 and 70 –100 ms following the stimulation, respectively Therefore, 1M – 3M, whose peak latencies were less than 50 ms and whose ECD were estimated in the SI, should be generated by the signals ascending through A-b fibers Since the 1M and 2M were unchanged during sleep, they were considered to be the primary responses In contrast, the 3M was reduced in amplitude during sleep Desmedt et al [31] reported that the middle-latency SEP components longer than 40 ms in latency were enhanced in amplitude when using a cognitive task (oddball paradigm) Our finding was compatible with theirs and we believe that the 3M relates to cognitive function to some degree The amplitudes of 4M and 5M were significantly larger in the painful than non-painful session, and are therefore considered to be pain-related components Since they were much reduced in amplitude during sleep, they should strongly relate to cognitive function All previous studies on pain-related SEF reported activation in SII in the bilateral hemispheres (see recent reviews by Kakigi et al [32,33]), but several recent studies reported activation of SI in the hemisphere contralateral to the stimulation, and that its latency was almost the same as for SII activity [34 – 37] Tarkka and Treede [38] first reported such a finding on analyzing SEP recordings by BESA Our findings were compatible with theirs We now speculate that signals ascending through A-d fibers reach both SI and SII directly from the thalamus This is very different from the processing of signals ascending through A-b fibers in which SI was activated first and then SII The role of SI in pain perception is yet to be elucidated We now speculate that SI is mainly responsible for the localization of stimulus or painful sites and the bilateral SII is responsible for the primary processing of pain perception—that is, how painful it is, what kind of painful feeling it is, and so on However, Inui et al [39] very recently found a shorterlatency SI activity, which appeared approximately 68 ms before the SII activity, following A-d fiber stimulation using an intra-epidermal needle Therefore, this important issue should be left for a future study A pain-related later component, whose peak latency was approximately 250 ms, was reported when the probe was centered at the vertex (Cz position) [3] We could not place Table Results using BESA Awake Stage Stage Early-component (nAm) 38.3 ^ 16.2 17.0 ^ 11.0* 15.4 ^ 11.3* (ms) 84.0 ^ 9.6 97.3 ^ 13.4 102.4 ^ 22.7 Late-component (nAm) 24.1 ^ 11.0 6.1 ^ 6.8* 3.5 ^ 5.5* (ms) 156.8 ^ 14.9 167.0 ^ 22.6 165.0 ^ 22.0 The strengths and latencies of components of the SII source while awake, in stage sleep and in stage sleep following painful electrical stimulation *P , 0:001 compared with the awake state (ANOVA followed by post-hoc paired comparisons using Fisher’s PLSD procedure) R Kakigi et al / Sleep Medicine (2003) 493–507 505 Fig Multiple source analysis using BESA was applied to the SEF waveforms following painful stimulation in the same subject as shown in Fig A twosource model (SI and SII) was the most appropriate The location and orientation of sources were the same while awake, in stage sleep and in stage sleep Both SI and SII activities were much reduced in amplitude or disappeared during sleep Adopted from Ref [4] 506 R Kakigi et al / Sleep Medicine (2003) 493–507 the probe at the Cz position when subjects lay on the bed asleep, and therefore did not discuss this component in the present report However, in our previous study on the effects of sleep on pain-related SEP [40], the pain-specific later component P240, whose peak latency was compatible with the SEF component generated in the cingulate cortex, was much reduced in amplitude or disappeared during sleep Therefore, pain-specific activities generated in the cingulate cortex were probably much reduced or disappeared during sleep Another useful method for recording pain-related SEF is a low power and long wavelength CO2 laser beam, which induces sensations of pain or heat when applied to the skin From studies in normal subjects and in patients with various types of sensory impairment, it has been established that CO2 laser stimuli cause the excitation of nociceptive receptors in the skin and that their signals ascend through small myelinated A-d fibers of the peripheral nerves and are probably mediated through the spinothalamic tract (see reviews by Kakigi et al [32,33]) In addition, we recently established a new method for selectively stimulating C fibers (see a review by Kakigi et al [41]) and reported the peripheral conduction [42], spinal conduction [43,44] and responses at the cerebral cortex [36,45] We are investigating the effects of sleep on C-fiber related SEP [45] and SEF [5], and would like to summarize the results in detail in a future review article General discussion and conclusion We reported the changes of brain responses during sleep following auditory, visual, somatosensory and painful somatosensory stimulation Surprisingly, very large changes were found under all conditions, although the changes in each were not the same However, there are some common findings Short-latency components, reflecting the primary cortical activities generated in the primary sensory cortex for each stimulus kind, show no significant change, or are slightly prolonged in latency and decreased in amplitude These findings indicate that the neuronal activities in the primary sensory cortex are not affected or are only slightly inhibited during sleep By contrast, middle- and longlatency components, probably reflecting secondary activities, are much affected during sleep Auditory components are markedly enhanced Waveforms of visual components differ very much, but principally they are enhanced By contrast, somatosensory components generated in SII are much reduced or disappear Since the dipole location is changed (AEF), unchanged (SEF) or vague (VEF) between the state of being awake and asleep, different regions responsible for such changes of activity may be one explanation, although the activated regions are very close to each other The enhancement of activities for auditory and visual systems probably indicates two possibilities: an increase in the activity of excitatory systems during sleep or a decrease in the activity of some inhibitory systems that are active in the awake state We have no evidence to support either, but prefer the latter since it is difficult to consider why neuronal activities would be increased during sleep The finding that waveforms of VEF become simpler during sleep may indicate that a number of inhibitory mechanisms are present for visual perception while awake; as mentioned in the discussion of each stimulus, some of them disappear during sleep Not only excitatory but also inhibitory mechanisms to avoid over-reaction should be necessary while awake, but such inhibitory systems not have to work during sleep Decreased activity in SII following somatosensory stimulation during sleep is an interesting finding There have been several reports on the effect of attention or distraction on SII activities The activities were much increased by applying attention to the stimulus [46], but were much decreased by distraction, for example a calculation task [26,45,47] Therefore, it seems reasonable that SII activities are much decreased or disappear during sleep SII in humans probably has higher integrative functions for somatosensory perception, and such a role is not necessary during sleep References [1] Naka D, Kakigi R, Hoshiyama M, et al Structure of the auditory evoked magnetic fields during sleep Neuroscience 1999;93: 573 –83 [2] Okusa T, Kakigi R Structure of visual evoked magnetic field during sleep in humans Neurosci Lett 2002;328:113 –6 [3] Kitamura Y, Kakigi R, Hoshiyama M, et al Effects of sleep on somatosensory evoked responses in human: a magnetoencephalographic study Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 1996;4:275–9 [4] Wang X, Inui K, Qiu Y, et al Effects of sleep on pain-related somatosensory evoked magnetic fields in humans Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 2003;17:388–99 [5] Qiu Y, Inui K, Wang X et al Effects of distraction on MEG responses ascending through C-fibers in humans Clin Neurophysiol, in press [6] Sarvas J Basic mathematical and electromagnetic concepts of the biomagnetic inverse problem Phys Med Biol 1987;32:11–12 [7] Rechtschaffen A, Kales A A manual for standardized terminology, techniques and scoring for sleep stages in human subjects Washington, DC: Public Health Service US Government Printing Office; 1968 [8] Chen BM, Buchwald JS Midlatency auditory evoked responses: differential effects of sleep in the cat Electroencephalogr clin Neurophysiol 1986;65:373–82 [9] Steriade M, Oakson G, Ropert N Firing rates and patterns of midbrain reticular neurons during steady and transitional states of the sleep– waking cycle Exp Brain Res 1982;46:37–51 [10] Wesensten NJ, Badia P The P300 component in sleep Physiol Behav 1988;44:215–20 [11] Merzenich MM, Brugge JF Representation of the cochlear partition of the superior temporal plane of the macaque monkey Brain Res 1973;50:275–96 [12] Romani GL, Williamson SJ Tonotopic organization of the human auditory cortex Science 1982;216:1339– 40 [13] Ciganek L Variability of the human visual evoked potential: normative data Electroencephalogr clin Neurophysiol 1969;27:35–42 R Kakigi et al / Sleep Medicine (2003) 493–507 [14] Apkarian P, Mirmiran M, Tijssen R Effects of behavioural state on visual processing in neonates Neuropediatrics 1991;22:85–91 [15] Whyte HE, Pearce JM, Taylor MJ Changes in the VEP in preterm neonates with arousal states, as assessed by EEG monitoring Electroencephalogr clin Neurophysiol 1987;68:223 –5 [16] Livingstone MS, Hubel DH Effects of sleep and arousal on the processing of visual information in the cat Nature 1981;291:554–61 [17] Sherman SM Tonic and burst firing: dual modes of thalamocortical relay Trends Neurosci 2001;24:122– [18] Steriade M, McCormick DA, Sejnowski TJ Thalamocortical oscillations in the sleeping and aroused brain Science 1993;262:679–85 [19] Felleman DJ, Van Essen DC Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex Cereb Cortex 1991;1:1–47 [20] Brecelj J, Kakigi R, Koyama S, Hoshiyama M Visual evoked magnetic responses to central and peripheral stimulation: simultaneous VEP recordings Brain Topogr 1998;10:227 –37 [21] Nakamura M, Kakigi R, Okusa T, et al Effects of check size on pattern reversal visual evoked magnetic field and potential Brain Res 2000;872:77– 86 [22] Lamme VA, Zipser K, Spekreijse H Figure-ground activity in primary visual cortex is suppressed by anesthesia Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1998;95:3263 –8 [23] Kitamura Y, Kakigi R, Hoshiyama M, et al Pain-related somatosensory evoked magnetic fields Electroencephalogr clin Neurophysiol 1995;95:463– 74 [24] Kitamura Y, Kakigi R, Hoshiyama M, et al Pain-related somatosensory evoked magnetic fields following lower limb stimulation J Neurol Sci 1997;145:187 –94 [25] Hoshiyama M, Kakigi R After effect of transcutaneous electrical nerve stimulation (TENS) on somatosensory evoked magnetic Clin Neurophysiol 2000;111:717– 24 [26] Yamasaki H, Kakigi R, Watanabe S, Hoshiyama M Effects of distraction on pain-related somatosensory evoked magnetic fields and potentials following painful electrical stimulation Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 2000;9:165– 75 [27] Hoshiyama M, Kakigi R, Koyama S, et al Activity in posterior parietal cortex following somatosensory stimulation in man: magnetoencephalographic study using spatio-temporal source analysis Brain Topogr 1997;10:23– 30 [28] Watanabe S, Kakigi R, Koyama S, et al Pain processing traced by magnetoencephalography in the human brain Brain Topogr 1998;10: 255–64 [29] Nihashi T, Kakigi R, Kawakami O, et al Representation of the ear in human primary somatosensory cortex Neuroimage 2001;13: 295–304 [30] Naka D, Kakigi R Simple and novel method for measuring conduction velocity of Ad fibers in humans J Clin Neurophysiol 1998;15:150–3 [31] Desmedt JE, Huy NT, Bourguet M The cognitive P40, N60 and P100 components of somatosensory evoked potentials and the earliest electrical signs of sensory processing in man Electroencephalogr clin Neurophysiol 1983;56:272–82 507 [32] Kakigi R, Hoshiyama M, Shimojo M, et al The somatosensory evoked magnetic fields Prog Neurobiol 2000;61:495–523 [33] Kakigi R, Watanabe S, Yamasaki H Pain-related somatosensory evoked potentials J Clin Neurophysiol 2000;17:295–308 [34] Ploner M, Schmitz F, Freund HJ, Schnitzler A Parallel activation of primary and secondary somatosensory cortices in human pain processing J Neurophysiol 1999;81:3100–4 [35] Kanda M, Nagamine T, Ikeda A, et al Primary somatosensory cortex is actively involved in pain processing in human Brain Res 2000;853: 282–9 [36] Tran TD, Inui K, Hoshiyama M, et al Cerebral activation by the signals ascending through unmyelinated C-fibers in humans: a magnetoencephalographic study Neuroscience 2002;113: 375–86 [37] Inui K, Tran TD, Qiu Y, et al Pain-related magnetic fields evoked by intra-epidermal electrical stimulation in humans Clin Neurophysiol 2002;113:298 –304 [38] Tarkka IM, Treede RD Equivalent electrical source analysis of painrelated somatosensory evoked potentials elicited by a CO2 laser J Clin Neurophysiol 1993;10:513 –9 [39] Inui K, Tran TD, Qiu Y, et al A comparative magnetoencephalographic study of cortical activations evoked by noxious and innocuous somatosensory stimulations Neuroscience 2003;120: 235–48 [40] Wang X, Inui K, Qiu Y, Hoshiyama M, Tran TD, Kakigi R Effects of sleep on pain-related somatosensory evoked potentials in humans Neurosci Res 2003;45:53–7 [41] Kakigi R, Tuan TD, Qiu Y, et al Cerebral responses following stimulation of unmyelinated C-fibers in humans: electro- and magneto-encephalographic study Neurosci Res 2003;45:255–75 [42] Tran TD, Lam K, Hoshiyama M, Kakigi R A new method for measuring the conduction velocities of Ab-, Ad- and C-fibers following electric and CO2 laser stimulation in humans Neurosci Lett 2001;301:187 –90 [43] Qiu Y, Inui K, Wang X, et al Conduction velocity of the spinothalamic tract in humans as assessed by CO2 laser stimulation of C-fibers in men Neurosci Lett 2001;311:181–4 [44] Tran TD, Inui K, Hoshiyama M, et al Conduction velocity of the spinothalamic tract following CO2 laser stimulation of C-fiber in humans Pain 2002;95:125– 31 [45] Qiu Y, Inui K, Wang X, et al Effects of attention, distraction and sleep on CO2 laser evoked potentials related to C-fibers in humans Clin Neurophysiol 2002;113:1579–85 [46] Mima T, Nagamine T, Nakamura K, Shibasaki H Attention modulates both primary and second somatosensory cortical activities in humans: a magnetoencephalographic study J Neurophysiol 1998; 80:2215–21 [47] Yamasaki H, Kakigi R, Watanabe S, Naka D Effects of distraction on pain perception: magneto- and electro-encephalographic studies Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 1999;8:73–6

Ngày đăng: 25/01/2022, 11:53