Tài liệu hạn chế xem trước, để xem đầy đủ mời bạn chọn Tải xuống
1
/ 15 trang
THÔNG TIN TÀI LIỆU
Thông tin cơ bản
Định dạng
Số trang
15
Dung lượng
107,15 KB
Nội dung
8 Familyenvironmentandpsychosis Pekka Tienari and Karl-Erik Wahlberg Introduction As with all forms of illness and disability, the primary burden of care for those with an enduring mental illness frequently falls on the family. Undoubtedly, without the ongoing support provided by close family, the impact of schizophrenia and other psychoses on the lives of many sufferers would be greatly increased. Families can provide emotional and practical support, continuity of care, and much needed help in negotiating interactions with professional mental health services. It is no surprise, then, that suggestions that families cause schizophrenia, as some asserted in the 1960s and 1970s (see Hirsch and Leff, 1975), were met with disbelief and anger. Singer and Wynne (1965) stated in their paper concerning the family relationships of patients with schizophrenia that biological and hereditary factors do contribute to disturbed transactions in some families. The majority of pioneers of family dynamic research did not, in fact, assert that parents were responsible for the psychosis in their children. Instead, they thought that many parents themselves were in need of help. However, many relatives felt that they were being blamed; they felt hurt, alienated and negatively labelled (Hatfield et al., 1987). It is important, therefore, that any discussion of families, the family environ- ment andpsychosis begin with three important disclaimers. First, as noted above, families frequently provide invaluable support and shoulder a large burden of care. Second, families (and specifically mothers) do not cause schizophrenia or any other psychosis. Third, the overwhelming majority of families provide positive, nurturing environments. However, this should not, and does not, lead to the conclusion that familyenvironment is irrelevant in understanding the onset and course of psychosis. Hirsch and Leff (1975), in their early review, concluded that there was a need for further research to test the more modest propositions that difficult and problematic family environments may increase the risk of schizo- phrenia and other psychoses, and worsen the course of these disorders. Society and Psychosis, ed. Craig Morgan, Kwame McKenzie and Paul Fearon. Published by Cambridge University Press. # Cambridge University Press 2008. In this chapter, we aim to provide an overview of research relating to familyenvironmentand schizophrenia and other psychoses. The most frequently studied intrafamilial factors are family communication and ‘expressed emotion’ (EE), and we will focus specifically on these in assessing the evidence for the role of familyenvironment in the aetiology and course of psychosis. Within the scope of this chapter, it is not possible to discuss fully the methodological issues in the study of familyenvironmentand psychosis. A thorough book is available on this topic (Jacob, 1987). History: early studies Apart from the question of the aetiology of schizophrenia, early studies of the families of patients with schizophrenia made two major contributions to our understanding of family dynamics: first, their focus on the family group was an important advance over traditional, individual-focused approaches; second, through the development of specific concepts, such as ‘double bind’, ‘fragmenta- tion’ and ‘pseudo-mutuality’, they alerted us to certain potentially important phenomena in family life (Mishler and Waxler, 1965). Goldstein and Strachan (1987) reviewed in detail the different aspects of earlier family research in relation to schizophrenia. They concluded that the large number of cross-sectional family studies has shown one consistent finding: the parents of patients with schizophrenia tend to show a deficit in their ability to share a focus of attention, to adopt the perspective of another person, and to communicate meaning clearly and accurately. But only in the area of communi- cation accuracy have cross-sectional comparison studies revealed consistent differ- ences between families of patients with schizophrenia and other families. They further concluded that disturbed communication and negative affect in families precede the development of schizophrenia spectrum disorders. It is, however, doubtful whether these findings are specific to schizophrenia. Further, their review suggested that family factors affect the course of schizophrenia, and that there is considerable continuity between the negative affective factors that precede the onset of the disorder and those that are predictive of course and outcome. To disentangle these issues further, they emphasised the value of high-risk longitudi- nal designs. After the initial enthusiasm in the 1950s and 1960s, research on family dynamics declined, partly for the reasons outlined above. However, investigations on intra- familial communication, EE and the efficacy of family interventions have been actively continued. In addition, interest in the interplay of genetic and environ- mental factors has been maintained. It is to this more recent research that we now turn. 113 Familyenvironmentandpsychosis Familial communication in schizophrenia Early research, as noted above, hinted at there being unclear and ambiguous communication patterns in the families of patients with schizophrenia. Research findings appear to confirm a greater prevalence of communication disturbance in the parents of schizophrenic patients than in other parents (e.g., Docherty et al., 1998; Goldstein, 1987a). There is further evidence that siblings of those with schizo- phrenia have more communication problems than other children (Docherty et al., 2004). These findings have been interpreted as suggesting that communication disturbances are indicators of genetic vulnerability in parents (e.g., Subotnik et al., 2002). However, this seems to be too simplified an explanation of the origin of disturbed communication patterns. The ‘communication deviance’ (CD) construct and scale were developed by Wynne and Singer (Singer et al., 1978) to describe the disturbed communi- cation styles found in the families of patients diagnosed with schizophrenia. Communication deviance is a scale that measures the degree to which a person does not share and maintain a focus of attention when communicating with other persons (Singer and Wynne, 1966), and to which meanings are not consensually or visibly validated. The scale consists of 42 categories adapted from the Singer–Wynne Rorschach scoring manual (Singer and Wynne, 1966, unpublished 1986 version, by M. T. Singer and L. C. Wynne). The most frequent and influential items of the CD scale are ‘abandoned, abruptly ceased, uncorrected remarks’, ‘inability or failure to verify own responses’, ‘odd sentence construction’ and ‘reiteration’. Communication deviance has most often been measured from standardised, verbatim Rorschach test records, and the essential issue is not the intrapsychic phenomena as traditionally interpreted, but more the pattern of reciprocal shared perceptions, focusing on attention and meaning in the communication process. In one study using this construct, in a subsample of the Finnish Adoptive Family Study of Schizophrenia, Wahlberg et al. (1997) found that adoptees at a high genetic risk, and with high-CD adoptive parents, had more sub-syndromal thought disorder than low-risk adoptees with high-CD adoptive parents. The high-risk adoptees with low-CD adoptive parents had slightly lower levels of sub-syndromal thought disorder than the low- risk adoptees of low-CD adoptive parents, findings which are suggestive of a gene– environment interaction and sensitivity of the genetic high-risk adoptees to the familyenvironment (see below). Further, Wahlberg et al. (2004) provide more evidence of gene–environment interaction in schizophrenia. They found that high- risk adoptees with high-CD adoptive parents had more mental disorders than low- risk adoptees with high-CD adoptive parents. Again, high-risk adoptees with low-CD adoptive parents had the same incidence of mental disorders as low-risk adoptees with low-CD adoptive parents. These findings support the biopsychosocial concept 114 P. Tienari and K.-E. Wahlberg of an interaction between genetic vulnerability or sensitivity and risk factors in the family rearing environment. The form of communication style considered here is likely to hinder a child’s cognitive development (especially in logical thinking and problem solving). These findings further suggest that more common communication styles support the cognitive development of high-risk children, perhaps protecting the vulnerable from schizophrenia. These results suggest that schizophrenia has both a genetic and an environmental base, and that neither alone can fully account for the illness. Origins of communication deviance It has been proposed that some children, exhibiting strange and incomprehensible behaviour indicative of an increased genetic risk for schizophrenia, evoke in their parents more negative and inconsistent ways of communicating (Bell, 1968). This may well be true in some cases, but it is questionable whether a child’s behaviour can evoke such changes in parents in the long term. For example, levels of CD have been found to be stable during adulthood over an 11 year time span (Wahlberg et al., 2001). Communication deviance also remains stable when a person is tested either alone or with his or her spouse and child (Keskitalo, 2000). Wahlberg et al. (1997) have further found that levels of CD in parents of adoptees at high genetic risk are similar to those of parents of adoptees at low genetic risk. This suggests that behavioural manifestations of genetic vulnerability for schizophrenia in adopted children do not provoke communication disturbances in parents. In the Wahlberg et al. (2004) study, all adoptees were mentally healthy at the time the CD of the adoptive parents was measured. The presence of mental disorder in the adoptees was measured 11 years later. Putting these findings together, we can assume that CD is a stable trait and not simply a reflective product of a disturbed child. The stability of CD indicates that high parental CD scores contribute to an enduring atmosphere in the family, which may shape cognitive function, especially the thought processes of a developing child. Expressed emotion Expressed emotion is a measure of familyenvironment that is based on how the parents or other carers talk about their ill relative. Based on the Camberwell Family Interview (CFI) (Vaughn and Leff, 1976), a long and detailed tape-recorded semi- structured interview, parents or other carers are classified as being high in EE if they make more than a specified number of critical comments or show any signs of hostility or marked emotional over-involvement. Positive aspects of the relationship may also be measured in the form of positive comments – a frequency count – and 115 Familyenvironmentandpsychosis warmth, a scaled score taking into account attitudes and comments evidenced throughout the interview. However, it is the dimensions of criticism, hostility and emotional over-involvement that are used to determine high and low levels of EE (Barrowclough and Hooley, 2003). In some studies, scoring has been based on a five- minute speech sample (Magana, 1990; Marom et al., 2005). Kavanagh et al. (1997) have developed a 30-item self-completed questionnaire for relatives, called the Family Attitude Scale. However, the CFI remains the gold standard. Expressed emotion has been found to be a reliable psychosocial predictor of relapse in schizophrenia (e.g., Butzlaff and Hooley, 1998). Marom et al. (2005) demonstrated the prolonged (up to seven years) predictive validity of EE in respect to psychiatric hospitalisation. There is also a growing literature concerning the role of EE in unipolar depression, bipolar disorder and eating disorders. Indeed, rather than being a construct of interest solely with respect to schizophrenia, EE is a more general predictor of poor outcome across a range of conditions. Furthermore, EE is a construct that is modifiable (Butzlaff and Hooley, 1998). In the University of California at Los Angeles (UCLA) High Risk Project (Goldstein, 1987b), EE preceded and predicted the development of schizophrenia spectrum disorders (communication deviance and affective style were even stronger predictors). It seems that EE may have some role in the aetiology of psychosis, even though its ability to predict relapse has been more strongly confirmed. Evidence is accumulating to show that high-EE and low-EE relatives may differ in the beliefs they hold about the patient and the problem behaviours associated with the patient’s illness. In particular, several studies have shown that critical relatives are more likely to hold patients responsible for their difficulties (Barrowclough and Hooley, 2003). Compared with low-EE relatives, high-EE relatives appear more conventional in their behaviour (higher norm favouring) and less satisfied with themselves and their lives (lower self-realisation). High-EE relatives may also be less flexible and tolerant and rate lower on empathy and achievement through independ- ence than low-EE relatives. In a study by Hooley and Hiller (2000), even after potential demographic confounders were controlled, inflexibility remained a signi- ficant predictor of high-EE. Heikkila ¨ et al. (2006), in a study of consecutive first- episode patients from a defined geographic area, found evidence that good cognitive functioning in patients may be associated with higher EE scores in relatives, a finding that may reflect the higher expectations (and consequent disappointments) of rela- tives of intellectually more able children. In another study, Patterson et al. (2005) found that a high-EE relationship involving high emotional over-involvement and critical comments may have adaptive functions, for example, in enabling the relative to respond to the crisis of a family member developing a psychosis. Kuipers et al. (2006) found that patients whose carers showed high EE had significantly higher levels of anxiety and depression, but not more psychotic 116 P. Tienari and K.-E. Wahlberg symptoms or lower self-esteem. Thus, their research hypothesis suggesting that the effect of high EE is mediated through affective changes in patients was partially supported. Even at the time of the first episode, the carers’ psychological appraisal, rather than the patient’s illness, appears to be influential in determining high EE (Raune et al., 2004). Family interventions Studies of the efficacy of family interventions for those with psychosis provide further relevant evidence. The Schizophrenia Patient Outcome Research Team, funded by the US National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) (Lehman et al., 1998), suggested that individual and group psychotherapies adhering to a psycho- dynamic model andfamily therapies based on the premise that family dysfunction is of primary importance in the aetiology of the schizophrenia should be avoided. Psychotherapists andfamily therapists did not accept this view. Many felt that the recommendations were not based on a full exploration of the field of psycho- logical therapies (Silver and Larsen, 2003). They have since been modified. The Schizophrenia Patient Outcome Research Team, in their updated treatment recom- mendations, stated that: ‘Persons with schizophrenia and their families who have ongoing contact with each other should be offered a family intervention, the key elements of which include a duration of at least nine months, illness education, crisis intervention, emotional support, and training in how to cope with illness symptoms and related problems.’ (Lehman et al., 2004, p. 202.) Pitschel-Walz et al. (2001) and Pilling et al. (2002) have both published meta- analyses of family intervention studies. Both included randomised controlled trials only. Pilling and colleagues, basing their review on ‘intention to treat’ analyses, found that family intervention reduced relapse rates and re-hospitalisations, and improved compliance with medication. Alanen (2004) has described the implementation of family interventions for patients with schizophrenia in Finland. Correspondingly, family interventions in bipolar disorders have been described in detail by Miklowitz and Goldstein (1997) and Callahan and Bauer (1999). The cause-and-effect relationships between mood disorder symptoms andfamily environmental factors are bidirectional; families are affected by the bipolar disorder as much as they affect it. There is no convincing evidence that disturbed family relationships cause the onset of bipolar disorder. In the absence of longitudinal high risk research, it is not possible to establish, with any certainty, causal connections between features of the childhood family envi- ronment and the eventual onset of bipolar disorder (or schizophrenia or other psychoses) in adulthood. Stierlin et al. (1986) reported their observations from a study of families, each with a young adult member who had been diagnosed as 117 Familyenvironmentandpsychosis manic-depressive (and, in a separate sample, with schizoaffective disorder). All families could be described as extremely rigid and bound-up systems. Many of them were characterised by a ‘restrictive parental complementarity’ and by recip- rocal delegation. Families with a relative with manic-depression showed both similarities and differences when compared with those with a relative with schizo- phrenia or a serious psychosomatic disorder. The authors also describe the therapeutic problems and the typical phases of the family therapeutic process (Weber et al., 1988), and report on a follow-up study in which they found a reduced relapse rate compared with the earlier histories of these patients with bipolar or schizoaffective psychoses (Retzer et al., 1991). Lindelo ¨ w (1999) showed that indicators of a lack of positive parent–child interactions, based on maternal reports for children at 10 years old, were significantly associated with (non- psychotic) depression in the children some 20 years later. Interplay of genes and the familyenvironment Rutter (2005a) has recently reviewed research on environmental risks for mental health and psychopathology. He underlined the need to differentiate between risk indicators and risk mediators, and the need to better understand the kinds of environmental influences that have major risk effects. Another challenge is to identify the origins of environmental risk factors and to find out whether they lie in gene–environment correlations (see Chapter 5), societal elements or personal experiences. It is also a challenge to determine the changes in the organism that provide the basis for the persistence of environmental effects on psychological functioning or psychopathology. Rutter (2005b), in summarising the main findings, emphasised the point that environmentally mediated risk effects are both real and important. Some effects operate predominantly on individual differences in liability, but some mainly affect the level of a trait or the rate of a disorder. The effects operate within a wide range of environments (and not just at the very extremes). They derive from influences in the family, school, peer group and community, as well as in the prenatal environment. The effects are often dependent on genetic susceptibility, i.e., they operate through gene–environment interactions. In an editorial, Arrindell and Perris (1999) underlined the importance of parental rearing behaviour because of its relevance for attachment and its later influence on personality development and mental health. In the Northern Finland 1966 Birth Cohort, unwanted pregnancy was found to be associated with an increased risk of schizophrenia. This factor may operate directly or may be a marker for other risk factors. Furthermore, first-born sons had an increased risk of schizophrenia in this cohort. However, some factors that have been found to be associated with schizophrenia in other studies were not replicated in this cohort: 118 P. Tienari and K.-E. Wahlberg living in a single-parent family, size of the family of origin and antenatal depres- sion in the mother (Isohanni et al., 2005). These results cannot be easily dismissed because they come from the study of a birth cohort, and thus are not as prone to the problems of bias and reverse causality common to case-control and other retrospective studies. To an important degree, genetic effects on behaviour arise either because they influence the extent to which the individual is likely to be exposed to environmental risk or because they affect the individual’s susceptibility to environmental adversities (Rutter, 2002; van Os and Sham, 2003). Gene–environment interaction can be defined as genetic control of sensitivity to environmental factors, or environmental control of gene expression (Kendler and Eaves, 1986). Thus, individuals with some genotypes are more likely than others to develop a disorder in the event of exposure to certain environmental factors. Given the existence of gene–environment inter- actions, it is possible that disorders cluster in families not just because of direct genetic effects, but also because relatives are more vulnerable to the risk-increasing effect of a prevalent environmental risk factor (van Os and Marcelis, 1998). A moderator (genotype) specifies on whom or under what conditions a mediator, such as rearing environment, will produce a certain outcome (Kraemer et al., 2001). Adoption studies of schizophrenia In adoption studies, genetic and rearing factors can be differentiated because, simply, the biological parents are not the rearing parents. Earlier adoption studies have convincingly confirmed the importance of a genetic contribution to schizophrenia (Kendler et al., 1994; Kety et al., 1994; Rosenthal et al., 1971, 1975). They have not, however, adequately examined the environmental contribution; they did not, for example, incorporate observations on rearing family environments. Rosenthal et al. (1975) conducted retrospective interviews with a group of adoptees about how they recalled their parent–child relationships. Wynne et al. (1976) pointed out that parental psychopathology has often been used as a rearing variable, although parental psychopathology is not necessarily relevant to the quality of their parenting. One aim of the Finnish Adoption Study was to investigate whether genetic risk for schizophrenia moderates the effects of adoptive rearing families (Tienari et al., 2003, 2004; Wahlberg et al., 1997, 2004). A national sample of adopted-away offspring of biological mothers with schizophrenia spectrum disorders was blindly compared with control adoptees of biological mothers with no diagnosis or non- schizophrenia spectrum diagnosis. The biological parents were diagnosed using personal structured interviews or hospital records. Adoptive families were assessed at initial phase with joint and individual interviews and psychological tests. High- risk adoptees (n ¼ 190) had a biological mother with a schizophrenia spectrum 119 Familyenvironmentandpsychosis disorder. Low-risk adoptees (n ¼ 192) had a biological mother with a non- spectrum or no disorder. Adoptees were personally interviewed a median of 12 years after the initial interview by a new psychiatrist blind to initial evaluations. All subjects (n ¼ 1741) were followed up 21 years later using national registers. The age-corrected morbid risk for (narrowly defined) definite DSM-III-R schizophrenia in low-genetic-risk adoptees reared in ‘healthy’ families was 0% (no genetic risk and no environmental risk), while that in high-genetic-risk adoptees (indexed by having a biological mother with a schizophrenia spectrum disorder) reared in ‘healthy’ families was 1.49% (genetic risk in the absence of environmental risk). The morbid risk for schizophrenia was 4.84% in low-genetic- risk adoptees reared in ‘dysfunctional’ families (environmental risk in the absence of genetic risk), but 13.04% in high-genetic-risk adoptees reared in ‘dysfunctional’ families (families with both a genetic risk and an ‘environmental risk’ present) (Tienari et al., 2002). The family’s overall health was rated by the interviewing psychiatrist using the Global Health Pathology Scale ranging from healthy (01) to pathological (99) (see Tienari et al., 2005). Disordered rearing in adoptive families predicted schizophrenia spectrum dis- orders in adoptees in a 21-year follow-up. However, it was only for adoptees at genetic high risk that a significant association between the measure of adoptive family functioning and the adoptee’s spectrum diagnosis was found. A similar result was obtained when the subgroup of adoptees with schizophrenia spectrum diagnoses at initial assessment was excluded. These findings indicate that adoptees at high genetic risk are more sensitive than adoptees at low genetic risk to both adverse and protective environmental effects in their rearing environments. This supports the hypothesis of interaction between the genotype and the environment (Tienari et al., 2004). In the same study, family ratings were made, based on joint family interviews in their homes using the Oulu Family Rating Scale. In the Oulu Family Rating Scale, 33 scales from a variety of sources were selected for ratings of specific aspects of adoptive family functioning (Tienari et al., 2004, 2005), each defined operationally at five levels from ‘healthy’ to ‘severely dysfunctional’. Some of the scales, bor- rowed from family researchers, had generalised applicability for the study of family relationships, for example, ‘non-acknowledgement’ and ‘manifest anxiety’. Other scales were constructed to tap concepts that have been hypothesised as especially relevant to families with schizophrenic members, for example, ‘narrow range of affect’ and ‘disrupted communication’. Extensive pilot testing and statistical evaluations of the ratings were carried out to examine inter-rater reliability and raters’ consensual understanding of the items. The family interviewers rated families both from personal observation and from tape recordings of one another’s interviews. Finally, nine scales were deleted as psychometrically unsatisfactory and 120 P. Tienari and K.-E. Wahlberg the remaining 24 were factor analysed into three groups, labelled as ‘critical or conflictual’, ‘constricted’ and ‘boundary problems’. These ratings were then used to assess the putative family rearing environments separately and jointly with the diagnoses of the proband birth mothers. The results showed that adoptees at high genetic risk were more sensitive to problems in the rearing adoptive family, in terms of both the three domains (factor groups) indicative of problematic adoptive rearing, i.e., ‘critical or conflictual’, ‘constricted’ and ‘boundary problems’, and the total score of the three domains (Tienari et al., 2004). An alternative way to view the findings is that there appears to be a protective effect of having been reared in a ‘healthy’ adoptive family with a low Oulu Family Rating Scale rating. High-genetic-risk adoptees reared in families with low Oulu Family Rating Scale scores had significantly fewer schizophrenia spectrum outcomes than high-genetic-risk adoptees with high Oulu Family Rating Scale scores. The joint effect of high genetic risk and a ‘dysfunctional’ rearing familyenvironment is essentially equal for each of the three groups that were differentiated in the factor analysis. This finding suggests that there is no specific, sharply delimited form of family environmental problem, and that the psychosocial environment has multiple components, perhaps similar to multifactorial genetics. Conceptually, the findings support the hypothesis of significant genotype by environment interaction, as defined by Kendler and Eaves (1986), implying genetic control of sensitivity to the environment or environmental control of genetic expression. In other words, a moderator (genotype) specifies on whom or under what conditions a rearing environment is associated with outcome (schizophrenia spectrum disorder in adoptees). Carter et al. (2002) demonstrated that gene–environment interaction was the most powerful predictor of schizo- phrenia in their multivariate analyses. The results are also in accordance with the hypothesis of Gottesman and Bertelsen (1989), which proposes that discordance in monozygotic twins may primarily be explained by the capacity of the schizo- phrenic genotype or diathesis to be unexpressed, unless released by some kind of environmental, including non-familial, stressors. The results support the popular diathesis–stress model of the aetiology of psychopathology. In this model, environmental stressors are hypothesised to have a particularly deleterious effect only on the individuals with a genetic diathesis, or a predisposition to a particular psychopathology (Rosenthal, 1963). Theoretically, one can hypothesise the possibility of an evocative gene–environment correlation if the child’s genetically influenced characteristics play a role in shaping his or her environ- ment. Reciprocal bi-directional effects between rearing parents and their children almost certainly take place. One would then expect adoptive families with geneti- cally high-risk adoptees to differ from those with genetically low-risk adoptees. However, high versus low genetic risk adoptees did not generate any such difference 121 Familyenvironmentandpsychosis [...]... diagnosis than parental psychiatric diagnosis Conclusions and future directions There is clear and accumulating evidence that the early familyenvironment is of aetiological importance in schizophrenia and, perhaps, other psychoses This is not a return to the simplistic and unthinking family blaming of the past It acknowledges that the emotional environment of childhood can have lasting effects on development... schizophrenia and related disorders: a further analysis of data from a longitudinal, prospective study In Understanding Major Mental Disorder: The Contribution of Family Interaction Research, ed K Hahlweg and M J Goldstein New York: Family Process Press, pp 11–32 Goldstein, M J (1987b) The UCLA High-Risk Project Schizophrenia Bulletin, 13, 505–14 Goldstein, M J and Strachan, A M (1987) The familyand schizophrenia... challenging and expensive REFERENCES Alanen, Y O (2004) The need-adapted approach – developing an integrated and individualized psychotherapeutically oriented treatment of schizophrenic patients Archives of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, 6, 5–21 Arrindell, W A and Perris, C (1999) Parental influences do matter! Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 100, 249–51 Barrowclough, C and Hooley, J M (2003) Attributions and. .. potential preventive measures In terms of educating health personnel, increasing knowledge about family functioning and the provision of supervision 123 Family environment and psychosis for working with whole families would help with the earlier identification of problems Screening problem families from a birth cohort, and careful evaluation of their functioning, would help develop methods that are useful in... In Family Interaction and Psychopathology: Theories, Methods and Findings, ed T Jacob New York: Plenum Press, pp 481–508 Gottesman, I I and Bertelsen, A (1989) Confirming unexpressed genotypes for schizophrenia Risks in the offspring of Fischer’s Danish identical and fraternal discordant twins Archives of General Psychiatry, 46, 867–72 124 P Tienari and K.-E Wahlberg Hatfield, A B., Spaniol, L and. .. 2003 Schizophrenia Bulletin, 30, 193–217 ¨ Lindelow, M (1999) Parent–child interaction and adult depression: a prospective study Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 100, 270–8 125 Family environment and psychosis Magana, A (1990) Manual for Coding Expressed Emotions from the Five Minute Speech Sample Los Angeles, CA: UCLA Family Project Marom, S., Muniz, H., Jones, B P et al (2005) Expressed emotion: relevance... Schizophrenia Bulletin, 31, 751–8 Miklowitz, D J and Goldstein, M J (1997) Bipolar Disorder: A Family- Focused Treatment Approach New York: The Guilford Press Mishler, E G and Waxler, N E (1965) Family interaction processes and schizophrenia: a review of current theories The Merrill-Palmer Quarterly of Behavior and Development, 11, 375–415 Patterson, P., Birchwood, M and Cocrane, R (2005) Expressed emotion as... schizophrenic person and the benefits of the psychotherapies – seeking PORT in the storm The Journal of American Academy of Psychoanalysis and Academic Psychiatry, 31 (1) (special issue), 1–268 Singer, M and Wynne, L C (1965) Thought disorder andfamily relations of schizophrenics Archives of General Psychiatry, 12, 210–12 Singer, M and Wynne, L C (1966) Principles for scoring communication defects and deviances... parents of schizophrenics: Rorschach and TAT scoring manuals Psychiatry, 29, 260–88 Singer, M., Wynne, L C and Toohey, M L (1978) Communication disorders and the families of schizophrenics In The Nature of Schizophrenia: New Approaches to Research and Treatment, ed L C Wynne, R L Cromwell and S Matthysse New York: John Wiley and Sons, pp 499–511 126 P Tienari and K.-E Wahlberg Stierlin, H., Weber,... disorders Family Process, 25, 325–36 Subotnik, K L., Goldstein, M J., Nuechterlein, K H et al (2002) Are communication deviance and expressed emotion related to family history of psychiatric disorders in schizophrenia? Schizophrenia Bulletin, 28, 719–29 Tienari, P., Wynne, L C., Sorri, A et al (2002) Genotype -environment interaction in the Finnish adoptive family study – interplay between genes and environment? . by Miklowitz and Goldstein (1997) and Callahan and Bauer (1999). The cause -and- effect relationships between mood disorder symptoms and family environmental. nurturing environments. However, this should not, and does not, lead to the conclusion that family environment is irrelevant in understanding the onset and course