Does Self-Selection Explain the Relationship Between Built Environment and Walking Behavior Empirical Evidence from Northern California

53 2 0
Does Self-Selection Explain the Relationship Between Built Environment and Walking Behavior  Empirical Evidence from Northern California

Đang tải... (xem toàn văn)

Tài liệu hạn chế xem trước, để xem đầy đủ mời bạn chọn Tải xuống

Thông tin tài liệu

Does Self-Selection Explain the Relationship Between Built Environment and Walking Behavior? Empirical Evidence from Northern California Susan Handy University of California, Davis Department of Environmental Science and Policy One Shields Avenue Davis, CA 95616-8762 Phone: 530-752-5878 Fax: 530-752-3350 E-mail: slhandy@ucdavis.edu Xinyu Cao University of California, Davis Department of Civil and Environmental Engineering One Shields Avenue Davis, CA 95616-8762 E-mail: xycao@ucdavis.edu Patricia L Mokhtarian University of California, Davis Department of Civil and Environmental Engineering One Shields Avenue Davis, CA 95616-8762 Phone: 530-752-7062 E-mail: plmokhtarian@ucdavis.edu Submitted to Journal of the American Planning Association Revised June 2005 Revised again July 2005 Does Self-Selection Explain the Relationship Between Built Environment and Walking Behavior? Empirical Evidence from Northern California ABSTRACT Suburban sprawl is increasingly being blamed for growing levels of obesity in the U.S The logic is simple: low-density, segregated-use suburbs are designed for driving rather than walking, leading people to drive more and walk less, thereby contributing to a decline in physical activity and an increase in weight The available evidence is less than conclusive, however: studies have established correlations between the built environment and walking but not a causal relationship Researchers are now debating the role of “self-selection” in explaining the observed correlations: residents who prefer to walk choose to live in more walkable neighborhoods? Using data from a survey of residents of eight neighborhoods in Northern California, this paper presents new evidence on the possibility of a causal relationship between the built environment and walking behavior This work makes two improvements on most previous studies: the incorporation of travel attitudes and neighborhood preferences into the analysis of walking behavior, and the use of a quasi-longitudinal design to test the relationship between changes in the built environment and changes in walking In both analyses, the results show that the built environment has an impact on walking behavior even after attitudes and preferences have been accounted for ii INTRODUCTION These days it’s hard to miss the fact that Americans are fatter than ever, and it’s almost as hard to miss the fact that suburban sprawl is being blamed in the media and in planning and public health circles for the obesity trend The logic is simple: low-density, segregated-use suburbs are designed for driving rather than walking, leading people to drive more and walk less, thereby contributing to a decline in physical activity and an increase in weight Indeed, recent studies show small but statistically significant correlations between suburban sprawl and obesity (McCann and Ewing 2003) and between time spent driving and obesity (Frank, et al 2004) The solution is therefore also apparently simple: design suburbs for walking rather than driving, leading people to walk more and drive less, thereby contributing to an increase in physical activity and a decrease in weight The evidence at first glance is persuasive but on closer examination is less than conclusive Studies have by now established a correlation between the built environment and walking behavior: residents of “walkable” neighborhoods walk more than residents of “non-walkable” neighborhoods (Saelens, et al 2003) But as any good textbook on research methods reminds us, correlation does not necessarily mean causality: a correlation between the built environment and walking behavior does not mean that a change in the built environment will lead to a change in walking behavior In particular, researchers are now debating the role of “self-selection” in explaining the observed correlations: residents who prefer to walk choose to live in more walkable neighborhoods? If so, planning still has an important role to play in creating environments that facilitate walking, especially to the extent that the supply of such environments is insufficient, a possibility suggested by Boarnet and Crane (2001) and supported empirically through surveys of developers and residents (Levine, et al 2002; Levine and Inam 2004) But the impact on those not already motivated to walk may be limited Using data from a survey of residents of eight neighborhoods in Northern California, this paper presents new evidence on the possibility of a causal relationship between the built environment and walking behavior, as well as biking behavior This work makes two improvements on most previous studies: the incorporation of travel attitudes and neighborhood preferences into the analysis of walking behavior, and the use of a quasi-longitudinal design to test the relationship between changes in the built environment and changes in walking In both analyses, the results show that the built environment has an impact on walking behavior even after attitudes and preferences have been accounted for LITERATURE REVIEW Two largely separate literatures provide evidence of a link between the built environment and walking Travel behavior research, based in the fields of transportation engineering, planning, and geography, has focused on walking as a mode of transportation – walking to reach a destination Physical activity research, based in the fields of psychology and public health, has focused on walking as a form of exercise A recent review of these two literatures found little consistency in the measures of the built environment or even the measures of walking used in the studies, making a direct comparison of their results difficult (Handy 2005) Nevertheless, certain patterns emerge Most notably, accessibility (measured in various ways) emerges as a strong correlate of walking behavior in both literatures, while the role of design variables is more ambiguous However, the results vary depending on the kind of walking: distance to destinations is more important for walking as a mode of transportation, while design appears to be more important for recreational walking Both literatures suggest that the built environment is not enough on its own to promote walking and may play a secondary role to personal factors The issue of causality has become one of the key questions in the debate over the link between neighborhood design and walking behavior Good scientific practice dictates three criteria for establishing causality between an independent variable (the cause) and a dependent variable (the effect): the cause and effect are statistically associated (association), the cause precedes the effect in time (time order), and no third factor creates an accidental or spurious relationship between the variables (non-spuriousness); many social scientists add a fourth criterion: the mechanism by which the cause influences the effect is known (causal mechanism) (Singleton and Straits 1999) Most studies so far have met the first criterion – statistical association – but have not met the other three Almost all of the available studies have used non-experimental cross-sectional designs that establish an association between the built environment and walking behavior However, these designs not establish whether the cause precedes the effect In addition, most studies have controlled for socio-demographic characteristics, thereby eliminating the possibility that income, for example, creates a spurious relationship between the built environment and walking behavior But few of these studies have accounted for the effects of attitudes towards walking, thereby ignoring the possibility that an association between attitudes and the chosen built environment and between attitudes and the choice to walk creates the appearance of a relationship between the built environment and walking By falling short on the criteria of time-order and nonspuriousness, these studies leave open the possibility of “self-selection,” in which individuals who prefer to walk choose to live in neighborhoods conducive to walking In this case, the characteristics of the built environment not cause them to walk more; rather, their desire to walk leads them to select a neighborhood with characteristics that enable them to walk more Although researchers have long recognized this limitation, moving beyond cross-sectional designs has been difficult, particularly when relying on readily available data from regional travel diary surveys Using data from the Puget Sound Transportation Panel to examine changes in travel behavior for residents who moved over a seven year period, Krizek (2000) found relatively weak correlations between changes in neighborhood design and changes in travel and in later analysis found a more convincing link between increases in accessibility and decreases in vehicle travel though not increases in walking (Krizek 2003) However, data on attitudes and preferences were not available With the 1994 Portland Travel Diary, Greenwald and Boarnet (2001) examined potential feedback between residential location choice and walking using instrumental variables to account for the influence on residential location choice of unobserved preferences possibly correlated with attitudes about walking Based on this analysis, they concluded that certain characteristics of the built environment promote walking, even taking into account the possibility of self-selection Using their own surveys, a few researchers have addressed the self-selection issue by directly accounting for preferences and attitudes, although with cross-sectional data as well In a study in Austin, TX, Handy and Clifton (2001) found significant differences in walking between neighborhoods of different types but qualitative evidence that residents selected neighborhoods in part based on their walkability Using structural equations modeling with data from the Bay Area to explore the relationships between neighborhood type and travel behavior, Bagley and Mokhtarian (2002) found that apparent associations between walking and neighborhood characteristics were largely explained by the self-selection of residents with certain attitudes and lifestyle preferences into certain kinds of neighborhoods On the other hand, Schwanen and Mokhtarian (2005), using more recent cross-sectional data from the Bay Area and focusing on the match (or mismatch) between preferred and actual neighborhood types, found that neighborhood type does exert some impact on travel behavior, even after attitudes are accounted for, but concluded that suburban environments have more of an effect than urban environments Using data from the Austin study, Cao, et al (2005) recently found that characteristics of the built environment influence both walking to the store and strolling around the neighborhood after accounting for a preference for neighborhoods conductive to walking Similarly, Khattak and Rodriguez (2005), using survey data for two neighborhoods in Chapel Hill, NC, found a significant difference in walking trips between a traditional neighborhood and a conventional surburban neighborhood after accounting for the effect of self-selection The causal mechanism that might link the built environment to walking has been given limited attention by researchers Boarnet and Crane (2001) offer an economic explanation: the built environment influences the price of travel, through its impact on travel time and other qualities of travel, which then influences the consumption of travel A similar idea is implicit in discrete choice models of travel behavior, in which individuals choose from some set of alternatives the one that maximizes their utility These models have been widely used to explain the choice of travel modes for a particular trip; in these applications, maximizing utility generally equates to minimizing travel time and other travel costs Applying this theory to walking is quite possible though not straight forward First, it is not clear that a decision to walk always represents a simple choice between walking and other modes While the travel behavior literature emphasizes the derived nature of travel demand, in which the demand for travel is derived from the demand for activities, this assumption does not necessarily hold for walking (or even for driving, for that matter; Handy, et al 2005; Mokhtarian and Salomon 2001) For example, the walk itself may be the motivation for a trip (Handy 1996), in which case the set of alternatives considered could include walking to the store, getting some other form of exercise, or forgoing exercise altogether Second, evidence on what factors most influence the utility of walking is relatively slim, given the limited range of characteristics of the built environment measured in most surveys, and the factors almost certainly vary depending on whether the walk or the destination is the motivation for the trip Even more challenging is the likelihood that residential location, attitudes and preferences, and walking behavior all interact with each other over time, as depicted in Figure If so, then different causal mechanisms may apply in different situations at different times, depending on the combination of the preferences of the individual and the type of environment in which she happens to live (Handy 2005) For example, for an individual with a high preference for walking who lives in a neighborhood conducive to walking, the built environment acts to enable the preferred behavior and reinforce preferences For an individual with a low preference for walking, living in a neighborhood conducive to walking might over time promote a preference for walking, which then leads to an increase in walking, or it might even be enough of an enticement to overcome a lack of preference for walking in the short term Alternatively, an individual who does not like to walk may come to believe that the environment is not conducive to walking as a way of rationalizing her behavior, in which case walking behavior exerts a causal effect on perceptions of the environment An individual who walks frequently, in contrast, has more direct experience with the environment and may have different perceptions of its suitability for walking – positive or negative – as a result These possibilities point to an important distinction between the built environment as it can be objectively measured and the built environment as perceived by residents; the relationship between the objective environment and the perceived environment is itself an important part of the puzzle METHODOLOGY Sorting out the relationships depicted in Figure requires a more sophisticated research design than was feasible for this study Our more limited objectives were, first, to test the association between the built environment and walking after accounting for attitudes and preferences, and second, to provide a stronger test of causality by examining the association between changes in the built environment and changes in walking Causal relationships are most validly established through experimental designs, in which individuals are randomized to treatment and control groups and behavior is measured for both groups before and after the treatment of interest (Singleton and Straits 1999) Neither randomization nor the application of a treatment is practical for studying the link between the neighborhood design and walking Instead, in this study, the treatment is defined as a move from one neighborhood to another and the lack of randomization is addressed by accounting for preferences and attitudes that might influence the choice of neighborhood The specific hypotheses addressed here are thus as follows: Differences in the built environment are associated with differences in walking, after accounting for socio-demographic characteristics and for attitudes and preferences More specifically, environments that offer better opportunities for walking are associated with more walking Changes in the built environment are associated with changes in walking, after accounting for socio-demographic characteristics and for attitudes and preferences More specifically, moves to environments that offer better opportunities for walking are associated with an increase in walking We selected eight neighborhoods in Northern California (Figure 2) that differ with respect to neighborhood design.1 The neighborhoods were selected to vary systematically on three dimensions: neighborhood type, size of the metropolitan area, and region of the state Neighborhood type was differentiated as “traditional” for areas built mostly in the pre-World II era, and “suburban” for areas built more recently (Figure 3) Although this design was intended to provide ample variation across neighborhood types, and these discrete indicators of neighborhood type are useful for descriptive comparisons, they are too simplistic for more detailed analyses For the multivariate models presented below, we used a rich set of variables describing the neighborhoods along a variety of dimensions Using data from the U.S Census, Figure Location of Neighborhoods 37 Figure Comparison of Traditional and Suburban Neighborhoods - Sacramento Sacramento – Traditional Street network Sacramento - Suburban Residential Streets Commercial centers 38 Figure Predicted Probabilities of Categories of Change in Walking as a Function of Changes in Accessibility 0.6 0.5 A lot less Predicted Probability 0.4 A little less About the Same A little more 0.3 A lot more 0.2 0.1 -4 -2 Changes in Accessiblity 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 Table 12 Change in Walking for Bay Area Neighborhoods from 19921 to 2003 Average Average Walk Percent Walking Strolling Frequency at Least Once Frequency (per 30 days) (per 30 days) (per 30 days) Mountain View Junior College Sunnyvale Rincon Valley 1992 2003 1992 2003 4.8 5.7 2.8 1.0 5.3 5.0 2.0 1.4 56% 64% 48% 33% 82% 73% 48% 37% * * Source for 1992 data is Handy 1996 *Statistically significant difference based on t-test or chi-square test 51 * Percent Strolling at Least Once (per 30 days) 1992 2003 1992 2003 10.1 12.6 11.6 10.8 9.7 10.6 8.0 8.3 78% 85% 78% 78% 89% 87% 84% 76% * * .. .Does Self-Selection Explain the Relationship Between Built Environment and Walking Behavior? Empirical Evidence from Northern California ABSTRACT Suburban sprawl... association between attitudes and the chosen built environment and between attitudes and the choice to walk creates the appearance of a relationship between the built environment and walking By... correlation does not necessarily mean causality: a correlation between the built environment and walking behavior does not mean that a change in the built environment will lead to a change in walking behavior

Ngày đăng: 20/10/2022, 06:03

Mục lục

  • 4. Findings

    • 4.1 Traditional versus Suburban Neighborhoods

      • 4.2 Cross-Sectional Analysis of Travel Behavior

      • 4.3 Quasi-Longitudinal Analysis of Travel Behavior

Tài liệu cùng người dùng

Tài liệu liên quan