Tài liệu hạn chế xem trước, để xem đầy đủ mời bạn chọn Tải xuống
1
/ 80 trang
THÔNG TIN TÀI LIỆU
Thông tin cơ bản
Định dạng
Số trang
80
Dung lượng
9,98 MB
Nội dung
EFFECTS OF POSTERIOR HYPOTHALAMIC
LESIONS ON FORMALIN-INDUCED
PAIN BEHAVIOURS
LIU LIMENG
NATIONAL UNIVERSITY OF SINGPAORE
2010
EFFECTS OF POSTERIOR HYPOTHALAMIC
LESIONS ON FORMALIN-INDUCED
PAIN BEHAVIOURS
LIU LIMENG
(B.SCI.(HONS), NUS)
A THESIS SUBMITTED
FOR THE DEGREE OF MASTER OF SCIENCE
DEPARTMENT OF PHYSIOLOGY
NATIONAL UNIVERSITY OF SINGPAORE
2010
ACKNOWLEGEMENT
I would like to express my thanks to the following people whose help and
support I greatly appreciate:
A/P Sanjay Khanna for giving me the opportunity to work in his laboratory.
For his patient guidance and assistance throughout the project and for the
mind-opening discussions without which this thesis would not have been
possible.
Esther for her assistance and encouragement throughout the project.
Zacky, Andy, Seok Ting, Jenn, and Brenda whom I have worked with, for
their assistance throughout the project and also for their company which made
the laboratory experience a truly enjoyable one.
i
TABLE OF CONTENTS
TITLE PAGE
ACKNOWLEDGEMENT
i
TABLE OF CONTENTS
ii
LIST OF FIGURES
v
LIST OF TABLES
viii
LIST OF ABBREVIATIONS
ix
LIST OF PUBLICATIONS
xi
SUMMARY
xii
INTRODUCTION
1
1.1 Functional organisation of the hippocampal formation
2
1.1.1 Hippocampus cytoarchitecture
3
1.1.2 Intrinsic connection within the hippocampus formation
6
1.1.2.1 Perforant pathway from the entorhinal
cortex to the dentate gyrus
7
1.1.2.2 The mossy fibres projection from the
dentate gyrus to CA3
7
1.1.2.3 The Schaffer collaterals to CA1
9
1.1.3 Extrinsic connection of hippocampus formation
11
1.1.3.1 Septo-hippocampal connections
11
1.1.3.2 Hippocampal-septal projection
12
ii
1.1.4 Hippocampal theta rhythm
1.2 Anatomy of supramammillary area (SuM)
13
15
1.2.1 Projections from SuM
16
1.2.2 Projections to SuM
18
1.2.3 SuM and hippocampal theta
19
1.2.4 Effects of manipulations of SuM on behaviour
and hippocampal theta
21
1.2.4.1 Spatial memory
21
1.2.4.2 Exploratory and defensive behaviour
22
1.2.4.3 Passive avoidance and Fixed Interval schedule
22
1.3 Pain
24
1.3.1 General description of pain
24
1.3.2 Pain transmission pathway
27
1.3.3 Formalin model of persistent pain
32
1.4 c-Fos
37
1.4.1 General description of c-Fos
37
1.4.2 Fos a an index of spinal nociceptive information processing
39
1.5 AMPA
41
1.6 Rationale and objectives of the present study
42
MATERIALS AND METHODS
44
2.1 Animals
45
2.2 Surgery procedure
45
2.3 Behavioural tests
46
iii
2.3.1 Scheduling of the behavioural tests
46
2.3.2 Open field test
46
2.3.3 Formalin test
47
2.4 Immunocytochemistry
51
2.4.1 Tissue preparation
51
2.4.2 Staining procedures
51
2.5 Data analysis
53
2.5.1 Quanitification of the size of the lesion in the brain
53
2.5.2 Quantification of Fos positive cells in
spinal cord and hippocampus
2.5.3 Statistical analysis
54
57
RESULTS
60
3.1 AMPA induced lesion in PH-SuM region
61
3.2 Effects of PH-SuM lesion on animal exploratory behaviour
76
3.3 Effects of PH-SuM lesion on animal behaviour
and spinal c-Fos in the formalin test
3.4 Effects of PH-SuM lesion on hippocampal c-Fos in the formalin test
77
91
DISCUSSION
100
4.1 Lesions
101
4.2 Open field
102
4.3 Formalin test
104
4.4 Summary and implications
108
iv
REFERENCES
109
LIST OF FIGURES
Figure 1
A three-dimensional organisation
hippocampal system in the rat brain.
Figure 2
The trisynaptic loop of the hippocampus.
Figure 3
Schematic
diagram
of
projections
from
supramammillary nucleus (SuM) to medial septum
(MS), lateral septum (LS) and hippocampus
17
Figure 4
Open field apparutus.
48
Figure 5
Formalin test apparatus.
50
Figure 6
Diagrammatic representation of lumbar L4 spinal cord
coronal section with laminar subdivisions.
56
Figure 7
Diagrammatic representation of anterior dorsal field
CA 1 (bregma -1.72 to bregma -4.65 mm Paxinos and
Watson, 2007).
58
Figure 8
Diagrammatic representation of posterior dorsal field
CA 1 and the ventral CA1 (bregma -4.65 to bregma 5.76 mm; Paxinos and Watson, 2007).
59
Figure 9
Digital images (500 dpi) of coronal sections of the
supramammillary (SuM) region showing microgliosis
following pretreatment with the immunotoxin AMPA.
62
Figure 10
Diagrammatic representation of the supramammillary
(SuM) region.
64
Figure 11
Diagrammatic representation of the largest (hatched)
and smallest (black and white checked) lesion at each
plane of section in each of the groups formed for
behavioural analysis.
65
Figure 12
Digitized images (500 dpi) of the lesion at each plane
of section in an animal with mSuM lesion
67
Figure 13
Illustration of the calculations to determine the extent
of the lesion.
70
v
of the
septo-
5
8
Figure 14
Illustration of the calculations to determine the extent
of the lesion against total volume of the
supramammillary region (SuM).
72
Figure 15
Illustration of the calculations to determine the extent
of the lesion in the medial and lateral supramammillary
regions (mSuM and SuML, respectively) against total
volume of the lesion.
73
Figure 16
The time course of the distance traveled upon exposure
to novel environment.
79
Figure 17
The total distance traveled over 20min in a novel
environment.
80
Figure 18
Different ambulatory behaviors in animals with lesion
in the medial and lateral supramammillary regions
(mSuM and SuML, respectively).
81
Figure 19
The time course of formalin-induced nociceptive
behaviors in animals with lesion of the medial or lateral
supramammillary region (mSuM and SuML,
respectively, on the figure) or the region dorsal to SuM
(Dorsal on the figure).
85
Figure 20
The total number of flinches (A), total duration of lick
(B), and total ambulation distance (C) in the second
phase of formalin test (11-60 min).
86
Figure 21
Digitized section (500 dpi) of lumbar (L4) spinal cord
showing expression of c-Fos positive cells (relatively
dark compared to the background) following right hind
paw injection of formalin (1.25%, 0.1ml).
87
Figure 22
The lack of effect of lesion of the medial and lateral
supramammillary region (mSuM and SuML,
respectively, on the figure) and of the region dorsal to
SuM (Dorsal on the figure) on c-Fos positive cell
counts from whole spinal cord (A) and the different
spinal laminae (B-E) ipsilateral to formalin injection.
90
Figure 23
c-Fos expression in anterior dorsal field CA1 following
injection of formalin (0.1 ml, 1.25%, subcutaneous into
right hind paw).
92
Figure 24
c-Fos expression in posterior dorsal field CA1 and
ventral CA1 following injection of formalin (0.1 ml,
1.25%, subcutaneous into right hind paw).
93
Figure 25
The lack of effect of lesion of the medial and lateral
95
vi
supramammillary regions (mSuM and SuML,
respectively, on the figure) and of the dorsal region
(Dorsal on the figure) on c-Fos positive cell counts
from hippocampal field CA1 to formalin injection
(0.1ml, 1.25%, right hind paw).
Figure 26
The lack of effect of lesion of the medial and lateral
supramammillary regions (mSuM and SuML,
respectively, on the figure) and of the dorsal region
(Dorsal on the figure) on c-Fos positive cell counts
from hippocampal field CA3 and dentate gyrus (DG) to
formalin injection (0.1ml, 1.25%, right hind paw).
96
Figure 27
C-Fos expression in dorsal field of the hippocampus
following subcutaneous injection of formalin (0.1 ml,
1.25%) into right hind paw.
97
Figure 28
The lack of lateralization of effect of lesion of the
medial and lateral supramammillary regions (mSuM
and SuML, respectively, on the figure) and of the
dorsal region (Dorsal on the figure) on c-Fos positive
cell counts from the hippocampus.
99
vii
LIST OF TABLES
Table 1
Extent of lesion within medial supramammillary and
lateral supramammillary against whole mSuM and the
whole unilateral SuML region, respectively.
68
Table 2
Extent of lesion of medial supramammillary and lateral
supramammillary against whole supramammillary.
74
Table 3
Extent of lesion of medial supramammillary and lateral
supramammillary against whole lesion.
75
Table 4
Summary of the groups of animals that were evaluated
for the effects of lesion of the posterior hypothalamus
(PH) - supramammillary (SuM) region on exploration.
78
Table 5
Summary of the groups of animals that were evaluated
for the effects of lesion of the posterior hypothalamus
(PH) - supramammillary (SuM) region on indices of
formalin pain.
82
Table 6
Summary of the groups of animals that were evaluated
for the effects of lesion of the posterior hypothalamus
(PH) - supramammillary (SuM) region on spinal c-Fos
in the formalin test.
88
Table 7
Summary of the groups of animals that were evaluated
for the effects of lesion of the posterior hypothalamus
(PH) - supramammillary (SuM) region on hippocampal
c-Fos in the formalin test.
94
viii
LIST OF ABBREVIATIONS
ABC
AMPA
AP-1
AP-1RE
BDNF
CaMKs
CHAT
CNS
CRE
CREB
DAB
DG
dpi
EC
ff
FLI
FI
GABA
HCN
IASP
IEG
i.p.
I/R
LS
MAPK
mf
mSuM
MS
MS-VLDBB
avidin-biotin-peroxidase complex
α-amino-3-hydroxyl-5-methyl-4isoxazole-propionate
activator protein-1
activator protein-1 response element
brain-derived neurotophic factor
calmodulin-independent protein
kinases
choline acetyltransferase
central nervous system
cAMP response element
cAMP-responsive element-binding
protein
diaminobenzidine treatment
dentate gyrus
dotes per square inch
entorhinal cortex
fimbria fornix
Fos-like immunoreactivity
fixed interval schedule test
gamma-amino butyric acid
hyperpolarization-induced cation
channel
International Association for the Study
of Pain
immediate early gene
NMDA
NK1
NS
OF
intraperitoneal
infrared
lateral septum
mitogen activated protein kinase
Mossy fibers
medial supramammillary
Medial septum
medial septal nuclei and the vertical
limb of diagonal band of Broca
N-methyl-D-aspartate
neurokinin 1
nociceptive specific
open field
O-LM
PAG
PDGF
PET
oriens-lacunosum moleculare
periaqueductal gray
platelet-derived growth factor
positron emission tomography
ix
PHA-L
PH-SuM
PKA
pp
PV
REM
RPO
sc
SIE
SuM
SuML
TRP
WDR
WGA-HRP
Phaseolus vulgaris leuco-agglutinin
Posterior hypothalamussupramammillary
protein kinase A
Perporant pathway
parvalbumin
rapid eye movement
rostral periolivary region
Shaffer collateral
sis-inducible element
supramammillary
Lateral supramammillary
transient receptor potential family of
ion channels
wide dynamic range
wheat germ agglutinin conjugated to
horseradish peroxidase
x
LIST OF PUBLICATIONS
ABSTRACT
Liu LM and Khanna S (2009) Effects of posterior hypothalamic
lesions on formalin-induced pain behaviours. Proceedings of the
International Australasian Winter Conference on Brain Research,
27: 4.43, Aug 29-Sep 2, Queenstown, New Zealand.
xi
SUMMARY
The posterior hypothalamus (PH)-supramammillary (SuM) region connects to
variety of regions in the CNS, some of which influence affective-motivational
behaviors. For example, the region is reciprocally connected to the
hippocampal formation. The hippocampal formation is crucial for adaptive
behaviors and, importantly, contributes to the negative affective-motivational
state during pain. In the present study we explored whether PH-SuM as well
modulate animal pain behaviors in the formalin model of persistent
inflammatory pain. Formalin (1.25%, 0.1ml) was injected subcutaneously into
the plantar surface of the right hind paw of PH-SuM lesioned or control nonlesioned animals. The lesion was induced by microinjection of the glutamate
receptor
ligand,
α-amino-3-hydroxyl-5-methyl-4-isoxazole-propionate
(AMPA, 0.99ng in 0.3µl).
The lesioned animals were subdivided into a
‘dorsal lesion group’ and a ventral lesion group based on the affected area
visualized using OX42 immunocytochemistry as a marker for microglia
activation. The damaged area in the dorsal lesion group included the PH and
adjacent areas, whereas the ventral lesion extended into the lateral SuM. The
lesioned area was bilateral or either ipsilateral or contralateral to the injected
paw. The lesion of SuM was incomplete. Neither dorsal nor ventral lesion
affected animal behavior in open field. Interestingly, ventral lesion
encompassing the lateral SuM attenuated animal behavior to formalin. The
effect was more marked during the later part of the formalin response leading
to truncated duration of formalin pain-induced behaviors. An effect was also
seen with dorsal lesion but was not as marked.
xii
These trends raise the
possibility that PH-SuM region, especially ventrally influence the affectivemotivational drive to pain.
xiii
INTRODUCTION
1
The supramammillary area (SuM) of the posterior hypothalamus has of late
gained prominence because of its modulation of hippocampal neural activity
(Pan and McNaughton, 2004). Indeed, it is part of an ascending relay from the
brainstem that influences hippocampal synaptic excitability and neural
synchronization via the medial septum. Further, much of the behavioural
effects observed with manipulation of SuM mimic that seen with medial sepal
and hippocampal manipulations.
Thus, the SuM is partly viewed as a
component of the neuraxis that also includes the septohippocampal formation.
The present thesis is built from this perspective and thus first describes the
septohippocampal region followed by a review of the SuM.
1.1 Functional organization of the hippocampal formation
The term hippocampal formation generally applies to the dentate gyrus, the
subicular complex (which is divided into three subdivisions: the subiculum,
pre-subiculum and parasubiculum), the entorhinal cortex (which is divided
into medial and lateral subdivisions) and the hippocampus proper (Amaral and
Witter, 1989). Hippocampus proper can be divided into three sub-fields, the
Cornu Ammonis fields CA1, CA2 and CA3. CA2 represents only a very
small portion of the hippocampus and its presence is often ignored in accounts
of hippocampal function (Johnston and Amaral, 1998).
2
The three-dimensional hippocampal formation in the rodent extends in a long
axis bending in a C-shaped fashion from the septal nuclei of the basal
forebrain rostrodorsally to the incipient temporal lobe caudoventrally (Amaral
and Witter, 1989). The long axis and orthogonal axis of the hippocampal
formation are referred as septotemporal axis and transverse axis, respectively
(Fig 1).
1.1.1 Hippocampus cytoarchitecture
There are two types of neurons in the hippocampal formation, namely
principal and non-principal neurons. The principal neurons include pyramidal
cells in CA fields and granule cells in the dentate gyrus. They form a narrow
compact cell layer (3-8 somta in thickness) and comprise about 90% of the
cell population in the hippocampal formation (Olbrich and Braak, 1985).
CA fields are structured depth wise in clearly defined dorsal-ventral strata: 1)
the alveus is the most superficial layer and contains the axons from pyramidal
neurons; 2) stratum oriens is the next layer below the alveus. The cell bodies
of inhibitory basket cells, horizontal trilaminar cells and the basal dendrites of
pyramidal neurons are located in this stratum; 3) stratum pyramidale contains
the cell bodies of the pyramidal neurons which are the principal excitatory
neurons of the hippocampus; 4) stratum radiatum which, like stratum oriens,
contains septal and commissural fibres. It also contains Schaffer collateral
fibres which are the projection fibres from CA3 to CA1. Some interneurons
including basket cells, bistratified cells, and radial trilaminar cells can also be
3
found here; 5) stratum lacunosum is a thin stratum that also contains Schaffer
collateral fibres and perforant path fibres from the superficial layers of
entorhinal cortex. Due to its small size, it is often grouped together with
stratum moleculare into a single stratum called stratum lacunosum-moleculare;
4
Figure 1. A three-dimensional organisation of the septo-hippocampal
system in the rat brain. The hippocampus is the C-shaped structure.
Abbreviations: fx = fornix; fi = fimbria; HC = hippocampus; MS = medial
septum (modified from Amaral and Witter 1995; picture adapted from (Ikonen,
2001).
5
6) stratum moleculare contains the perforant path fibres which form synapses
onto the distal, apical dendrites of pyramidal cells (Johnston and Amaral,
1998).
Granule cells in dentate gyrus have a cone-shaped dendritic tree towards the
hippocampal fissure, with all branches confined to molecular layer (a
relatively cell free layer). The three layers in dentate gyrus are as follow: 1)
the polymorphic layer is the most superficial layer of the dentate gyrus and is
often considered as a separate subfield; 2) stratum granulosum contains the
cell bodies of the dentate granule cells; 3) stratum moleculare is where both
commissural fibres from the contralateral dentate gyrus run and form synapses
as well as where inputs from the medial septum terminate, both on the
proximal dendrites of the granule cells (Johnston and Amaral, 1998).
The non-principal cells mainly refer to local inhibitory interneurons scattered
throughout the hippocampal formation (Olbrich and Braak, 1985). Examples
of such interneurons include the basket cells, horizontal trilaminar cells,
bistratified cells, and radial trilaminar cells. The interneurons play a critical
role in modulation of principal cell excitability and activity via local circuits
(Freund and Buzsaki, 1996).
1.1.2 Intrinsic connection within the hippocampus formation
A unidirectional intrinsic connection which consists of a series of excitatory
pathways enables a sequential flow of information through the hippocampal
6
formation. It is organized in a trisynaptic circuit, starting from the perforant
pathway from the entorhinal cortex to the dentate gyrus, continues with the
mossy fibres projection from the dentate gyrus to CA3 and ends with the
Schaffer collaterals to CA1 (Fig 2).
1.1.2.1
Perforant pathway from the entorhinal cortex to the dentate
gyrus
The perforant pathway is the major input to the hippocampus. The axons of
the perforant path arise principally in layers II and III of the entorhinal cortex,
with minor contributions from the deeper layers IV and V. Axons from layers
II/IV project to the granule cells of the dentate gyrus and pyramidal cells of
the CA3 region, while those from layers III/V project to the pyramidal cells of
the CA1 and the subiculum (Johnston and Amaral, 1998).
1.1.2.2 The mossy fibres projection from the dentate gyrus to CA3
The unmyelinated mossy fibres arise from granule cells and are organized in a
lamellar fashion, with little divergence in septotemporal axis (Amaral and
Witter, 1989). All mossy fibres extend throughout the full transverse extent of
CA3 field despite the location of granule cells of origin. They are selective in
their contacts in which each axon only contacts fourteen pyramidal cells
7
Figure 2. The trisynaptic loop of the hippocampus. The filled triangles
represent the pyramidal cell layer (CA1 and CA3) and the filled circles
represent the granular cell layer of the dentate gyrus. The trisynaptic circuit is
composed of (1) Perforant pathway (pp) fibres which arise from EC and
terminate on granule cells of DG, (2) mossy fibres (mf) of the granule cells
which contact pyramidal cells of field CA3, and (3) Schaffer collaterals (sc)
which synapse onto pyramidal cells in field CA1. Abbreviations: EC =
entorhinal cortex; DG = dentate gyrus; pp = perforant pathway; mf = mossy
fibres; sc = Schaffer collaterals; ff = fimbria fornix. (Picture adapted from
Ikonen, 2001).
8
(Claiborne et al., 1986). Most of mossy fibres terminate in the proximal
dendritic region of ipsilateral CA3 pyramidal cells.
1.1.2.3 The Schaffer collaterals to CA1
Schaffer collaterals arise from CA3 pyramidal neurons in ipsilateral
hippocampus and projects both in the septotemporal axis and in the transverse
axis topographically.
For instance, CA3 pyramidal cells located close to
dentate gyrus tend to project more heavily to the septal level of CA1 and near
the subicular border (Amaral, 1993) and terminate mostly in the superficial
stratum radiatum of CA1. On the other hand, those located close to CA2 tend
to project to temporal CA1 and terminate heavily in both stratum radiatum and
stratum oriens. The septotemporal divergence of Schaffer collateral from a
CA3 pyramidal cell is as much as three-fourths of the entire septotemporal
extent of CA1 (Ishizuka et al., 1990). In addition, CA3 pyramidal neurons
also send projections to CA1 pyramidal cells via the Schaffer collaterals and
join with the commissural fibres from the CA3 of the contralateral
hippocampus (Amaral, 1993).
The intrinsic connection between principal neurons and the interneurons is
mainly inhibitory in nature (Glickfeld et al., 2009), and different interneurons
exert their inhibitory effects over pyramidal cell excitability through different
mechanisms. There are two types of interneurons of the hippocampus, namely,
axo-axonic/somatic and axo-dendritic interneurons (Freund and Buzsaki,
1996). Chanderlier and basket cells belong to the first type while oriens-
9
lacunosum moleculare (O-LM), bistratified and trilaminar neurons are
examples from the latter type. The main axons of axo-axonic interneurons are
myelinated and run tangentially to their soma. They emit collaterals that
arborize and form dense multiple synapses in the prisomatic region of the
principal cells.
Axons of axo-dendritic neurons, on the other hand, are
unmyelinated and arborize upon reaching the target laminae. To discuss the
main difference in controlling pyramidal cells, basket cells and O-LM are used
as examples.
For basket cells, the axons arborise only within the stratum pyramidale and do
not penetrate the other layers of the hippocampus.
The axons form
symmetrical synapses with the soma and initial axon segments of the
neighbouring pyramidal cell (Sik et al., 1995). The dendritic tree of basket
cells arborises in the stratum oriens, pyramidale and lacunosum-moleculare
and receives inputs from neighbouring pyramidal cells (Freund and Buzsaki,
1996).
On the other hand, the dendritic tree of O-LM interneurons is confined to the
stratum oriens and alveus, while the axons penetrate the hippocampus and
arborise at the stratum lacunosum moleculare (Sik et al., 1995). The axons
were shown to perfectly overlap with the terminals of entorhinal afferents in
the stratum lacunosum moleculare, suggesting that O-LM interneurons could
exert an inhibitory regulation of entorhinal synapses terminating on the distal
dendrites of the pyramidal cells (Blasco-Ibanez and Freund, 1995; Katona et
al., 1999).
10
1.1.3 Extrinsic connection of hippocampus formation
Two types of inputs to hippocampal principle cells have been identified: 1)
laminated afferents from cortical areas (such as entorhinal area and amygdala)
terminating in the specific layers along the dendrites; 2) diffuse afferents from
various subcortical regions (such as medial septal nuclei and brainstem
monoaminergic nuclei) targeting all layers including perisomatic and dendritic
regions (Paxinos, 1985).
Given the focus of this study, only projections
between hippocampal and septal region are described.
1.1.3.1 Septo-hippocampal connections
Projections from neurons in the medial septum are mainly from the medial
septal nuclei and the vertical limb of diagonal band of Broca (MS-VLDBB).
Using anterograde tracer Phaseolus vulgaris leuco-agglutinin (PHA-L),
Nyakas et al. (1987) reported that the medial septal projections to the
hippocampus formation are organized in a topographic manner. For instance,
the dorsal field CA1 and dorsal dentate gyrus receive dense afferent input
from VLDBB and relatively less dense afferents from MS while the ventral
hippocampal subfields receive massive input from both MS and VLDBB. In
addition, the septo-hippocampal projections are dominantly ipsilateral in
nature (Monmaur and Thomson, 1983).
Moreover, the septohippocampal
efferents are identified as type 1 that are thick and coarse axons with large
terminal boutons, and type 2 that are thin axons with varicosities (Nyakas et
al., 1987).
11
There are at least three types of neurons identified in the medial septal region
by immunohistology and biochemical assays, namely cholinergic (Brashear et
al., 1986), GABAergic (Barry et al., 1985) and glutamatergic (Colom et al.,
2005) neurons.
A significant proportion of medial septal neurons are
cholinergic (30%-70%, depending on the rostrocaudal level; (Senut et al.,
1989). Indeed, many neurons in the medial septum that were labelled
following microinjection of retrograde tracer into the dorsal hippocampus
were immunoreactive for the acetylcholine synthesizing enzyme choline
acetyltransferase (CHAT), indicating that these were cholinergic in nature
(Kiss et al., 1990).
The septal cholinergic projection innervates both
pyramidal cells and interneurons with both symmetrical and asymmetrical
synapse (Frotscher and Leranth, 1985). The septal GABAergic projection, in
contrast, mainly target cell bodies or dendrites of hippocampal interneurons
(Acsady et al., 1993). Interestingly, the type 2 fibres of Nyakas et al. (1987)
correspond to septo-hippocampal GABAergic afferent (Freund and Antal,
1988). Glutamatergic neurons concentrate in the medial septal regions and are
distributed among the cholinergic and GABAergic neurons (Colom et al.,
2005)
1.1.3.2 Hippocampal-septal projection
The hippocampal-septal projection is the most prominent efferent projections
from the hippocampus among all the efferent projections from the region
(Leranth et al., 1992). The main subcortical target of this efferent is lateral
septum (Tamamaki et al., 1984). The hippocampal projections to the medial
12
septal region are relatively sparse and believed to be GABAergic (Toth and
Freund, 1992). It is notable that the projection from hippocampus to medial
septal region is from field CA2 and CA3, with less contribution from field
CA1 and the dentate gyrus (Gaykema et al., 1991).
In turn, evidence from tract tracing and double labelling suggests that
GABAergic and glutamatergic neurons at the border of the medial septum and
lateral septum project to SuM (Borhegyi and Freund, 1998; Leranth et al.,
1999; Kiss et al., 2002).
1.1.4 Hippocampal theta rhythm
Hippocampal theta rhythm is one of the several types of the extracellular field
potentials observed in field CA1 that reflect activity of network of local
neurons. It is a large amplitude sinusoidal rhythmic EEG pattern. Two types
of theta have been characterised according to the different behavioural and
pharmacological properties. Type 1 theta (~8 Hz) occurs during locomotion
and other types of voluntary behaviour and during rapid eye movement (REM)
sleep, and is unaffected by the anticholinergic drug atropine. Type 2 theta
(4~6 Hz) occurs during immobility and during anaesthesia induced by
urethane, and is abolished by administration of atropine (Kramis et al., 1975).
The extracellular theta waves recorded from CA1 pyramidal cell layer are
mirror images of the rhythmic intracellular oscillations of the pyramidal cell
somatic membrane potential (Leung and Yim, 1986; Soltesz and Deschenes,
13
1993; Ylinen et al., 1995). Here it is notable that the intracellular membrane
potential oscillations recorded from pyramidal cell soma are induced, at least
in part, by rhythmic inhibition impinging upon these neurons. Consistent with
this, the putative inhibitory interneurons in CA1 increase their discharge
around the positive phase of the local extracellular theta that temporally
corresponds to the somatic hyperpolarisation of the pyramidal cells (Buzsaki
and Eidelberg, 1983; Fox et al., 1986; Ylinen et al., 1995; Klausberger et al.,
2003) Conversely, the probability of action potential discharge of majority of
pyramidal cells is high around the negative phase of the local theta (Buzsaki
and Eidelberg, 1983; Fox et al., 1986; Ylinen et al., 1995; Csicsvari et al.,
1998). The negative phase corresponds to the release of pyramidal cells from
rhythmic inhibition. Furthermore, the phase-locked pattern of firing indicates
that the neurons of the hippocampus exhibit synchronized phasic firing during
theta.
The MS-VLDBB region is believed to be the pacemaker of the hippocampal
theta activity (Buzsaki and Eidelberg, 1983; Bland, 1986; Vinogradova, 1995).
The crucial role that MS-VLDBB plays in generation of hippocampal theta is
supported by a large body of evidence. These included (1) permanent lesions
or reversible blockade of the MS-VLDBB region eliminated hippocampal
theta in behaving rat (Buzsaki and Eidelberg, 1983); (2) inactivation of the
MS-VLDBB by local anaesthetic procaine or the anticholinergic drug atropine
attenuated reticularly-induced hippocampal theta (Jiang and Khanna, 2004; Li
et al., 2007). (3) lesion of either septal cholinergic neurons with the
immunotoxin 192 IgG-saporin or the parvalbumin (PV) positive septal
14
GABAergic neurons with the glutamate agonist kainic acid attenuated the
power of sensory- and exploration-induced theta in anaesthetized and
behaving animals, respectively (Lee et al., 1994; Zheng and Khanna, 2001;
Yoder and Pang, 2005). However, a recent study by Simon et al (2006)
demonstrated that, in urethane anesthetized rats and unanaesthetized restrained
rats, cholinergic neurons were slow-firing and did not display theta-related
bursting. This may suggest that cholinergic neurons play a more permissive
and modulatory role other than acting as pacemakers (Simon et al., 2006).
Indeed, Hangya et al (2009) argued that PV positive GABAeric neurons
expressing hyperpolarization-induced cation channel (HCN) may be the
putative pacemaker neurons leading the hippocampal network activity
(Hangya et al., 2009). In this study, the extracellular discharge of putative PV
positive GABAergic septohippocampal neurons that express HCN were
observed to precede the transition of hippocampal EEG from irregular activity
to theta rhythm in anaesthetized rat.
1.2 Anatomy of Supramammillary area (SuM)
SuM as a whole is found to connect to many forebrain areas, most of which
are the structures of limbic system such as amygdala, entorhinal cortex,
dentate gyrus, septum, preoptic area, anterior hypothalamus, locus coeruleus
(Saper et al., 1976; Ottersen, 1980; Swanson, 1982; Haglund et al., 1984;
Gonzalo-Ruiz et al., 1992b; Hayakawa and Zyo, 1994; Leranth and Kiss, 1996;
15
Borhegyi et al., 1998; Vertes and McKenna, 2000; Kiss et al., 2002). In
addition, the projections from SuM and to SuM are chemically very diverse.
In the following section, more focus will be given to connections pertinent to
hippocampus and septal area.
1.2.1 Projections from SUM
SuM as a whole contains calretinin-containing cells (Kiss et al., 2000),
dopaminergic cells (Swanson, 1982), and substance P containing cells
(Borhegyi and Leranth, 1997; Fig 3).
The most medial portion of SuM
(mSuM) contains small packed cells and is distinguished by having substance
P containing cells. Cells in this region have weak connections to hippocampus
and these connections originate mostly from substance P containing cells
(Borhegyi and Leranth, 1997; Fig 3). There is a high density of dopaminergic
cells in the mSuM (Swanson, 1982; Fig 3).
Retrograde tracers and
immunochemical techniques showed that these dopamininergic cells project to
lateral septum (Swanson, 1982; Shepard et al., 1988) and mammillary bodies
(Gonzalo-Ruiz et al., 1992a; Fig 3).
16
Hippocampus
MS
Calretinin
LS
LS
Substance P
Dopamine
SuML
mSuM
SuML
MM
Figure 3. Schematic diagram of projections from supramammillary
nucleus (SuM) to medial septum (MS), lateral septum (LS) and
hippocampus. Symbols , , ,represent cells with specific histochemical
markers such as calretinin, substance P and dopamine respectively, but with
no indication of co-localization of multiple markers. SuM as a whole contains
calretinin-containing cells, dopaminergic cells and substance P containing
cells. Medial SuM (mSuM) is distinguished by having substance P containing
cells which send projections to hippocampus. There is high density of
dopaminergic cells in mSuM and they project to LS and mammillary bodies
(MM). Cells in lateral portion of SuM (SuML) stain for calretinin and project
to hippocampus and MS/LS region.
17
The lateral portion of SuM (SuML) around principal mammillary tract
contains large cells. The cells become more loosely packed when moving
more laterally (Leranth and Kiss, 1996).
Cells in this region stain for
calretinin but not substance P. The great majority of the calretinin containing
cells were back-labelled with [3H]D-aspartate microinjected into the dentate
gyrus or the MS-VLDBB/LS region, suggesting that they project to dentate
gyrus and MS-VLDBB/LS and utilize aspartate/glutamate as neurotransmitters
(Kiss et al., 2000). In addition, the terminals of supramammillary axons,
labelled with the anterograde tracer PHA-L, were observed to innervate the
principal neurons in DG (Magloczky et al., 1994).
Apart from projecting to septohippocampus, by anterograde and retrograde
tracers and transmitter immunocytochemistry, SuM was also found to send
projections to LS, especially the dorsal and intermediate LS nuclei,
mammillary nuclei (especially lateral mammillary nucleus), thalamus,
posterior hypothalamus, amygdale, medial prefrontal and the entorhinal
cortices (Swanson, 1982; Shibata, 1987; Vertes, 1988; Hayakawa et al., 1993;
Risold and Swanson, 1997)
1.2.2 Projections to SuM
Injections of wheat germ agglutinin conjugated to horseradish peroxidase
(WGA-HRP) into the supramammillary nucleus resulted in retrogradely
labelled neurons in MS-VLDBB region and other regions including
infralimbic cortex, medial and lateral preoptic nucleus, subiculum, laterodorsal
18
tegmental nucleus, compact subnucleus of the central superior nucleus and the
dorsal raphe nucleus (Gonzalo-Ruiz et al., 1999). In the MS-VLDBB region,
most of the SuM projectiong neurons (80-85%) were found to be cholinergic
neurons whereas small numbers of them were identified as GABA-containing
neurons. In addition, GABAergic neurons which contain calretinin and/or
calbindin and located at the border between LS and MS region were found to
project to SuM (see section 1.1.3.2). Because these GABAergic neurons
receive input from the entorhinal cortex, the authors proposed a negative
feedback mechanism whereby entorhinal cortex influenced SuM neurons
through these neurons to modulate the SuM mediation of hippocampal theta
activity (Borhegyi and Freund, 1998; Leranth et al., 1999)
1.2.3 SuM and hippocampal theta
The SuM is implicated in modulation of the hippocampal synaptic activity and
theta activation. Indeed, in context of the latter, it has been proposed that the
SuM mediates the generation of hippocampal theta rhythm, at least partly.
Thus, Kirk and McNaughton (1991) found that mSuM cells discharge
rhythmically which is in phase with concurrent hippocampal theta activity.
Further, microinjections of the local anaesthetic procaine into posterior
hypothalamus blocked the reticularly-elicited rhythmic discharge of MS
neurons, but inactivation of the MS did not block the rhythmic bursting of
hypothalamic neurons (Bland et al., 1994; Kirk et al., 1996). Moreover, in
urethane anaesthetised rats, procaine injections into mSuM decreased both the
frequency and amplitude of reticularly-elicited theta (Kirk and McNaughton,
19
1993; Khanna et al., 2004). On the other hand, procaine microinjected more
rostrally into the medial forebrain bundle (that carries afferents from posterior
hypothalamus to medial septal region) or the medial septal region decreased
the amplitude only. As regards SuML, Jiang and Khanna (2004) found that
GABA, the ubiquitous inhibitory ligand, microinjected into SuML attenuated
the power of reticularly-elicited theta without having effects on frequency.
Similarly, microinjection of the nicotinic receptor antagonist, mecamylamine,
into the SuML reduced the power, but not frequency, of both reticular
stimulation- and sensory stimulation- induced theta (Ariffin et al., 2010).
However, this effect was not seen with mSuM microinjection of the antagonist
(Ariffin et al., 2010).
The SuM is also implicated in modulation of the hippocampal synaptic
activity. Glutamate stimulation of SuML produced a substantial increase in the
perforant path stimulation-induced population spike in the dentate gyrus
(Carre and Harley, 1991). Inactivation of the mSuM with GABA abolished
reticularly-elicited suppression of CA1 population spike, thus indicating that
SuM mediated reticularly-elicited suppression of CA1 pyramidal cell synaptic
excitability to CA3 stimulation (Jiang and Khanna, 2004).
Moreover,
microinjection of mecamylamine into the SuML, but not mSuM, attenuated
both reticularly-elicited and noxious stimulation induced population spikes in
CA1 suggesting that nicotinic cholinoceptive neurons in SuML is a possible
element of the neural drive to the hippocampus on rostral periolivary region
(RPO) activation and noxious stimulation (Ariffin et al., 2010). Taken together,
the above data showed that SuM as a whole modulates hippocampal theta and
20
population spike suppression.
In addition, there may be functional and
neurochemical heterogeneity along the mediolateral axis of SuM neurons in
controlling different aspects of hippocampal theta such as frequency and
amplitude.
1.2.4
Effects of manipulations of SuM on behaviour and hippocampal
theta
The effect of manipulations of SuM has been examined on a variety of animal
behaviors. Especially the focus of such investigations has been to determine
whether SuM manipulations affect hippocampal-mediated behaviors (see
sections 1.2.4.1 to 1.2.4.3) since SuM and the septohippocampal regions are
interlinked.
1.2.4.1 Spatial memory
Large lesions produced by microinjection of α-amino-3-hydroxyl-5-methyl-4isoxazole-propionate (AMPA) into mSuM (made prior to training) resulted in
only modest decrease in theta frequency (by about 0.4 Hz) without affecting
spatial reference memory (as assessed by averaging the distance swum to
reach the target on each of the 4 days of training) or swimming speed in the
Morris water maze (Pan and McNaughton, 2002). Consistently, infusion of a
benzodiazepine, that acts on the GABA-benzodiazepine-chloride-ionophore
complex to potentiate the effects of endogenous GABA (McNaughton and
Sedgwick, 1978), into SuM produced no obvious impairment of learning and
21
theta activity (Pan and McNaughton, 1997). Thus, SuM, unlike hippocampus,
seems not to play important roles in either spatial working memory or
reference memory. The conclusion is further supported by Sziklas and Petride
who reported that large lesions including destruction of medial mammillary
bodies and SuM, do not affect radial arm maze learning (Sziklas and Petrides,
1993).
1.2.4.2 Exploratory and defensive behaviour
Pan and McNaughton reported that neurotoxic lesions of mSuM increased
exploratory ambulation in open field test but did not reduce the average
frequency of theta, whereas lesions in SuML had no effect on exploratory
behaviour and theta (Pan and McNaughton, 2002). In fear conditioning test,
neurotoxic lesions of mSuM, but not unilateral SuML, decreased contextual
fear conditioning (i.e. decreased freezing and so increase movement) without
affecting conditioning to the discrete stimulus (a 10-KHz, 75-dB tone which
was present for 20s; Pan and McNaughton, 2002). Similarly, hippocampal
lesions impaired conditioning of fear to the context in which conditioning is
carried out but did not impair conditioning to the discrete fear stimulus itself.
(Phillips and LeDoux, 1992, 1994)
1.2.4.3 Passive avoidance and Fixed Interval schedule
According to Gray and McNaughton, passive avoidance is a test not only of
defense but also of behavioural inhibition - a postulated fundamental function
22
of the hippocampus (Gray, 1982; Gray and McNaughton, 2000). Generally,
the rat is allowed to move from the one compartment (e.g. white compartment)
to the other compartment (e.g. black). In the conditioning session the entry
into the black compartment is punished with a mild inescapable electrical
shock (Barrionuevo et al., 2000). The typical behavior with passive avoidance
on the test day is that the rat tends to delay its time in entering the black
compartment and spend more time in the white compartment. Neurotoxic
lesions of either mSuM or unilateral SuML produced an increase in
responding (i.e. reduced latency and increased time in black compartment) and
thus impaired inhibition (Pan and McNaughton, 2002).
Another test of behavioral inhibition is fixed interval schedule test (FI). In this
test, the rat is rewarded for the first desired response (i.e. bar press), after
which a set interval of time must pass before any responses will be rewarded.
For example, if a rat is on a fixed interval of 60 s, it will be rewarded for its
first bar press, but not again until after 60 s has passed - no matter how many
time it presses the bar. The typical pattern of responding with a fixed interval
schedule is to see most of the responding around the time at which the reward
is due. Then once the rat has received its reward for an interval, it usually
stops responding for most of the remainder of the interval (Ellen E Pastorino,
2008; Ellen and Susann, 2008). With FI, injections of a benzodiazepaine into
mSUM produced increased responding and a concomitant decrease in theta
frequency. The effects were even comparable to the effects of i.p. injection of
the benzodiazepine (Woodnorth and McNaughton, 2002b, a).
Moreover,
neurotoxic lesions of either mSuM or unilateral SuML increased responding
23
and so impaired inhibition. The change in behaviour produced by lesions of
mSuM was accompanied by a decrease in theta frequency of about 0.4 Hz
whereas lesions of SuML did not reduce theta frequency (Pan and
McNaughton, 2002). The behavioural effects were similar with hippocampal
lesions but greater when compared to either mSuM or SuML lesion (Manning
and McDonough, 1974).
Overall, the behavioural data discussed above indicated that SuM lesions
affected a number of hippocampal-dependent behaviours, except spatial
learning in Morris water maze. The effect on some behaviours, such as tests
of behavioural inhibition were accompanied by a decrease in theta frequency.
On the other hand, SuM lesion did not change theta frequency although it
increased animal ambulation in open field test. Similarly, large electrolytic
lesion of the SuM did not affect theta in freely moving animals (Thinschmidt
et al., 1995). Likely, therefore, exploration-induced theta may be mediated via
other pathways to the hippocampus.
1.3 Pain
1.3.1 General description of pain
Pain has been defined by the International Association for the Study of Pain
(IASP) as “an unpleasant sensory and emotional experience associated with
actual or potential tissue damage or described in terms of such damage
24
(Merskey, 1979).”
Thus, pain is a multifaceted and highly subjective
experience that is unique to each individual.
Indeed, McCaffrey and Beebe
described pain as “whatever the experiencing person says it is, existing
whenever the experiencing person says it does (McCaffrey and Beebe, 1989;
McCaffrey M., 1989)”. Due to its subjective nature, the word pain is not used
with reference to animals as they are unable to communicate verbally the pain
experience. Nonetheless, animals display overt and covert changes similar to
that in human upon exposure to noxious stimuli which induce (1) behavioural
responses, (e.g. flexion reflex) and voluntary reactions (e.g. escape and
avoidance), and (2) physiological responses, (e.g. change in heart rate and
release of stress hormones).
It should be noted that nociception and pain are somewhat different.
Nociception is a process in which nociceptors are activated by a noxious
stimulus. Nociceptors are free nerve endings found in most organs and tissues
in the body and activated by either a noxious mechanical (touch or pressure),
thermal (hot or cold), or chemical (endogenous or exogenous) stimulus
(Millan, 1999). Pain refers to a feeling or a perception of irritating, miserable
or unbearable sensations.
Pain can be caused without nociception.
For
instance, in the case of allodynia, a pathological condition of a painful
response to typically non-painful stimulus, patients can experience pain by a
light touch without nociceptors being activated (Merskey and Bogduk, 1994).
In the clinical setting, there are two broad categories of pain, acute and chronic.
Under these broad categories fall the subtypes of pain, which include
25
inflammatory, neuropathic, cancer etc. Acute pain is characterized by its short
duration, identifiable cause and self-limitedness.
It functions as an
endogenous protective mechanism that signals the brain of the occurrence of
real or potential tissue injury, thus prompting a protective response (Henry,
1962). The commonly used models to study acute pain include hot-plate, tailflick and tail-pinch tests, in which the subject is allowed to remove the
affected region from noxious stimuli by making a response (D'Amour and
Smith, 1941; Woolfe and MacDonald, 1944). The foregoing are models of
brief pain. One model used to study acute persistent pain is formalin model, in
which the subject must cope with pain/nociception due to hind paw injection
of the dilute formaldehyde (Albe-Fessard, 1990). Chronic pain is, on the other
hand, unrelenting, has no identifiable cause, spreads beyond the original site of
injury, and serves no biological function.
Three interactive components of pain have been identified by Melzack and
Casey (1968): sensory-discriminative, affective-motivational and cognitiveevaluative. The sensory discriminative component describes the intensity,
location, quality and duration of a noxious stimulus and these features are
encoded by the ascending pain pathways that transmits nociceptive
information from peripheral tissues to the cerebral cortex for interpretation as
pain.
The affective component of pain refers to the unpleasantness or
emotional distress that invariably attends the sensation of physical pain. It
encompasses a range of negative emotions such as anger, fear, sadness and
suffering and triggers the subjects to seek pain relief. Further, the affective
component is closely linked to the autonomic nervous system and the
26
associated cardiovascular, respiratory and gastrointestinal responses.
The
cognitive-evaluative components reflects the evaluation of the meaning and
possible consequences of pain based on prior experiences. This component
interacts intimately with the affective-motivational aspect of pain.
The neural systems of the sensory-discriminative of pain have been
extensively studied. However, relatively less attention has been paid to the
study of affective-motivational and cognitive characteristics of pain.
It’s
important to note that sensory-discriminative component of pain may utilize
different neural pathways, at least in part, from affective-motivational and
cognitive component. For example, in the case of demented patients with
impairment of affective and cognitive functions, such as Alzheimer’s disease,
the emotional response to painful stimuli was significantly reduced, though the
sensory-discriminative component of pain was maintained intact (Benedetti et
al., 1999). Furthermore, Rainville et al demonstrated using positron emission
tomography (PET; an imaging technique measuring cerebral blood flow) that
hypnotic suggestion that altered the unpleasantness but not the perceived
intensity of noxious heat stimulus resulted in significant change in painevoked activity in cingulated cortex but not primary somatosensory cortex
(Rainville et al., 1997).
1.3.2 Pain transmission pathway
This section briefly describes the transmission of nociceptive signal in the
central nervous system (CNS).
The signal, which is action potential, is
27
generated via the nociceptors or free nerve endings as a result of application of
noxious stimuli in their receptive fields. The signal is then conveyed along the
associated axons, namely myelinated Aδ axons, which conduct at 5-30 m/s, or
unmyelinated C fiber axons, which conduct at less than 2 m/s. In general, the
Aδ- and C-nociceptors supplying the skin are functionally classified as: Aδmechanosentive nociceptor (respond to intense mechanical stimuli); Aδmechanothermal nociceptor (respond to both intense mechanical and thermal
stimuli) and C-polymodal nociceptors (respond to thermal, mechanical and
chemical stimuli; Millan, 1999). However, more recent findings suggest a
greater complexity and phenotypical diversity, especially in relation to
behavior. Thus, for example, the C-fibers are subdivided based on presence or
absence of peptides and functionally destruction of the peptidergic C-fibers
with the neurotoxin capsaicin in mice attenuated animal behavioral response to
noxious heat, but not to noxious mechanical stimuli (Scherrer et al., 2009).
Conversely, genetic deletion of Nav1.8 expressing neurons, including the
destruction of non-peptidergic C-fibers but with sparing of population of
peptidergic C-fibers, strongly attenuated the animal response to noxious
mechanical stimulation, but not to noxious heat and non-noxious mechanical
stimulation (Abrahamsen et al., 2008).
Nociceptive afferents then enter the dorsal horn of the spinal cord where they
give off branches that contact second-order neurons. Aδ-fibers terminate in
laminas I and V while C-fibers terminate in lamina I and II (Besson and
Chaouch, 1987; Millan, 1999). More recently, Braz et al (2005) identified the
termination pattern of non-peptidergic C-fibers (which stain for isolectin IB4)
28
by way of genetic expression of wheat germ agglutinin (WGA) in these
nociceptors. These primary afferents mostly terminated in inner lamina II
above the layer of spinal interneurons positive for γ isoform of protein kinase
C.
In contrast, the peptidergic C-fibers terminate in lamina I, directly
contacting projection neurons that transmit nociceptive messages to brainstem
and/or thalamus as well as the interneurons in outer lamina II (Todd et al.,
2000; Scherrer et al., 2009).
Two distinct types of spinal second-order neurons are identified (Besson and
Chaouch, 1987; Millan, 1999): nociceptive specific (NS) neurons or high
threshold neurons, which respond only by nociceptive inputs, distributing
primarily in lamina I, and (2) wide dynamic range (WDR) neurons, which
respond to a broad range of stimulus, including both nociceptive and
innocuous low-threshold mechanical activation, distributing in deeper laminae
of dorsal horn. There is a visceral input to these WDR neurons which results
in convergence of somatic and visceral inputs in the phenomenon of referred
pain (Scherrer et al., 2009).
Projection neurons within laminar I constitute the major output from the dorsal
horn to the brain (Basbaum and Jessell, 2000).
Neuroanatomical studies
including anterograde tracing with PHA-L and retrograde tracer studies
provided evidence that lamina I neurons project heavily to thalamus especially
the ventroposterolateral nucleus (Gauriau and Bernard, 2004). Retrograde
labeling indicated that greater than 80% of the lamina I spinothalamic neurons
projected to the parabrachial area (Hylden et al., 1989). The nociceptive
29
responses of lamina I neurons projecting to the parabrachial area have been
characterized in halothane anaesthetized rat (Bester et al., 2000).
The
spinoparabrachial neurons were mostly nociceptive specific (~75%) to
noxious stimuli, including mechanical and thermal stimuli. The rest were
wide-dynamic range type that responded to both noxious and non-noxious
stimuli. The receptive field of these neurons was small, covering one to two
toes and their firing rate increased with the intensity of noxious stimuli
indicating that these played a role in intensity-encoding (Bester et al., 2000).
Moreover, NS and WDR neurons are identified in VPL/VPM and are
topographically organised (Albe-Fessard et al., 1985).
Both anterograde and retrograde tracing techniques have identified a number
of other ascending nociceptive pathways from the spinal neurons which are
believed to involve in affective-motivational component of pain.
These
include the spinoreticular (Fields et al., 1977; Peschanski and Besson, 1984;
Bester et al., 1995) and spinohypothalamic (Burstein et al., 1991; Giesler et al.,
1994) pathways, the spino-parabrachio-amygdaloid (Schaible and Grubb,
1993; Millan, 1999) and other ascending pathways to the brainstem regions
such as to ventrolateral medulla, the periaqueductal gray, and also further
rostrally to the septum (Fields et al., 1977; Menetrey et al., 1982; Peschanski
and Besson, 1984; Bester et al., 1995; Millan, 1999). Consistent with this,
transneuronal transfer of WGA expressed genetically in non-peptidergic Cfibers indicated that these afferents influenced a variety of brain regions via
spinal neurons (Braz et al., 2005). These brain regions include the amygdala,
30
ventromedial hypothalamus, bed nucleus of stria terminalis, globus palidus
and periaqueductal gray.
The nociceptive information eventually reaches a widespread cortical mantle.
Consistent to this, both NS and WDR nociceptive neurons are identified in
deep layers of the primary somatosensory cortex (Lamour et al., 1982;
Kenshalo and Isensee, 1983). These cortical nociceptive neurons have small
receptive fields that are arranged in a somatotopic pattern and code for
intensity and localization of noxious stimuli (Lamour et al., 1983). Indeed,
lesions in the sensory cortex and/or lateral thalamus impaired sensory
attributes, leading to poor intensity encoding and spatial localization of
noxious stimulus (Marshall, 1951; Treede et al., 1999).
More recently,
imaging techniques indicated a large distributed brain networks that was
activated during painful stimulation. For example, the cortical and subcortical
brain regions found to be commonly activated by nociceptive stimulation
included: anterior cingulated cortex, insula, frontal cortices, and S1, S2
(Peyron et al., 2004).
Nociceptive neurons have also been electrophysiologically recorded in limbic
structures, such as anterior cingulated cortex (Vogt et al., 1992), hippocampus
(Khanna, 1997) and amygdala (Bernard et al., 1992). Generally, nociceptive
neurons in the limbic system have little or no somatotopic organization with
large receptive fields. These limbic regions may receive spinal nociceptive
inputs via the ascending diffuse system polysynaptically, though some other
limbic structures and may also be directly accessed by spinal nociceptive
31
neurons (Burstein and Giesler, 1989; Burstein and Potrebic, 1993).
For
example, the hippocampus receives nociceptive inputs, at least in part, from
the medial septum such that lesion of the region attenuates the noxious
stimulus-induced theta and pyramidal cell suppression in hippocampus.
1.3.3 Formalin model of persistent pain
Most of the traditional tests of nociception, such as tail-flick and hot-plate
tests which are based on a phasic stimulus of high intensity, produce
nociceptive responses which are short-lasting. It is therefore not suitable for
the study of continuous pain. On the other hand, formalin test generates
nociceptive responses which are long-lasting, making it a good model to study
inflammatory persistent pain. Moreover, because formalin-induced pain is
produced by injured tissue, the test provides a more valid model for clinical
pain than the tests with phasic mechanical or thermal stimuli (Dubuisson and
Dennis, 1977; Abbott et al., 1981; Alreja et al., 1984).
Formalin is an aqueous solution of 37% (weight/weight) formaldehyde. In
formalin test, diluted formalin (0.5-5%, 20-100µl) is injected subcutaneously
into one of the paws (either the dorsal surface or the plantar; Dubuisson and
Dennis, 1977). The hind paw is usually chosen for injection because the
formalin-elicited response of the hind paw, such as licking, rarely occur during
normal grooming behaviour (Tjolsen et al., 1992). Injection of formalin is
generally made by lightly restraining the animal.
Immediately after the
injection, several reproducible and easy identifiable behavioural responses are
32
observed, which include lifting, licking, and flinching the affected paw
(Dubuisson and Dennis, 1977). In addition, autonomic changes are observed
that include an increase in blood pressure and heart rate following hind paw
injection of formalin (Taylor et al., 1995). Both the nociceptive behaviours
and the cardiovascular changes observed following injection of formalin are
expressed in a biphasic fashion with an acute phase (phase 1, 0 to 5 min) that
is followed by a tonic phase (phase 2, generally from 11 min to 60 min after
injection) with a quiescent period (interphase) of about 5 min in between the
two phases.
Injection of formalin evokes a concentration-dependent increase in nociceptive
behaviour with formalin concentration ranging from 0.625-5% (Khanna et al.,
2004; Taylor et al., 1995). The autonomic changes such as blood pressure and
heart rate (Taylor et al., 1995) as well as release of spinal nitric oxide
metabolites (nitrite/nitrate) and glutamate (Okuda et al., 2001) are also found
correlated highly with behaviours and are dependent on formalin
concentration in those studies. However, at a higher concentration (10%) of
formalin, nociceptive behavioural responses were decreased and failed to
produce a clear-cut release of spinal nitrite/nitrate and glutamate (Okuda et al.,
2001).
The nociceptive responses in the formalin test can be suppressed by nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drugs (Hunskaar et al., 1986; Drower et al., 1987),
systemically administrated opiate analgesic such as morphine (Abbott et al.,
1995), intraplantar injection of lidocaine derivative (Taylor et al., 1995) and
33
electrical stimulation of various regions of the brain including the brain stem
(Dubuisson and Dennis, 1977).
Of the two behaviours that are formalin concentration-dependent, namely
licking and flinching, the latter is regarded as a more robust parameter as
compared to licking which is more readily influenced by other factors such as
taste aversion of the formalin (Wheeler-Aceto and Cowan, 1991). Moreover,
implantation of chronic intrathecal cannula has been reported to completely
suppress licking but not flinching (Sawynok and Reid, 2001, 2003).
Administration of pentobarbital, a non-analgesic sedative, is found to affect
licking while sparing flinching (Abbott and Guy, 1995). In addition, Sawynok
showed that flinching and licking could be differentially modulated, indicating
that they may involve distinct mechanisms (Sawynok and Liu, 2003; Sawynok
and Reid, 2003). For example, systemic administration of amitriptyline, an
antidepressant, simultaneously increased flinching while suppressing licking
(Sawynok and Reid, 2001).
At the neural level, hind paw injection of formalin also elicited a biphasic
increase in activity of peripheral nociceptors (McCall et al., 1996; Puig and
Sorkin, 1996). More recent evidence suggested that formalin excited
nociceptors by directly activating TRPA1, a member of the Transient Receptor
Potential family of ion channels. Formalin induced robust calcium influx in
cells expressing cloned or native TRPA1 channels, and these responses were
suppressed by TRPA1 antagonist (McNamara et al., 2007).
Moreover,
behavioural studies showed that pharmacologic blockade or genetic ablation
34
of TRPA1 resulted in attenuated nociceptive responses in formalin pain model
(Macpherson et al., 2007; McNamara et al., 2007).
Consistent to a role of peripheral nociceptors in formalin pain, both the
formalin-induced behaviours and cardiovascular changes are blocked by
peripheral administration of local anaesthetic that presumably blocked afferent
input to the central nervous system (Coderre et al., 1990; Taylor et al., 1995).
More recent studies also provided evidence for role of peripheral nociceptors
in formalin nociception. Mice with their sensory neurons expressing the
sodium channel Nav1.8 destroyed showed suppressed pain responses in the
formalin test (Abrahamsen et al., 2008). Similarly, a reduction in formalin
induced pain behaviours was also observed in sodium channel Nav1.7
knockout mice (Nassar et al., 2004).
As in the periphery, hind paw injection of formalin evoked a biphasic increase
in discharge of dorsal horn neurons (Dickenson and Sullivan, 1987; Chapman
and Dickenson, 1995; Pitcher and Henry, 2002). Evidence suggested that
spinal excitation involved excitatory synaptic transmission that mediated, at
least in part, via the alpha-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid
(AMPA) and N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) glutamate receptors.
In this
regard, formalin injection elicited an increase in the spinal level of the
excitatory amino acid neurotransmitter, glutamate (Kantner et al., 1985;
Malmberg and Yaksh, 1995) while, conversely, the formalin-induced
discharge of spinal neurons was antagonized by intrathecal administration of
antagonist at the AMPA and NMDA glutamate receptors (Chapman and
35
Dickenson, 1995). Interestingly, formalin injection also evoked a persistent
increase in the size of the spinal field potential (i.e. potentiation) evoked on Cfibre stimulation in anaesthetized rat (Ikeda et al., 2008). This effect was
reproduced by low frequency stimulation of nociceptors that mimicked the
persistent activation of primary afferents following injection of formalin. The
potentiation was blocked by systemic administration of MK-801, an NMDA
receptor antagonist.
The authors proposed that the formalin-induced
potentiation in the spinal cord was akin to long-term potentiation of synaptic
transmission and might act as a synaptic pain amplifier mechanism (Ikeda et
al., 2008). Indeed, and consistent with a key role of spinal glutamatergic
transmission, intrathecal administration of antagonist at NMDA and AMPA
receptors prior to formalin injection eliminated or strongly reduced the phase 2
response (Coderre and Melzack, 1992; Malmberg and Yaksh, 1992).
At supraspinal level, injection of formalin activated a number of brain
structures. Functional imaging studies using PET techniques suggested that
formalin injected in left hind paw of rats activated hindlimb cortex,
retrosplenial portion of the cingulated cortex and midbrain periaqueductal gray
(PAG; Casey, 1999). Further, an increase in immediate early gene (IEG) (eg.
c-fos) expression was also observed in prefrontal cortex, locus coeruleus, PAG
and ventromedial nuclei following formalin injection into the hind paw of rats
(Aloisi, 1997; Aloisi et al., 2000).
In general, formalin test involves
integration of many brain regions which subserve sensory and emotional
processes.
36
1.4 c-Fos
1.4.1 General description of c-Fos
The term immediate-early gene (IEG) was first used for viral genes (Kovacs,
2008). These genes were also activated by the eukaryotic cells upon viral
invasion and were responsible for transcriptional reprogramming of the host to
promote virus replication.
Examples of IEGs are c-fos, c-jun and c-myc
(Kovacs, 2008).
Evidence suggested that c-fos was involved in regulating neuronal cell
excitability, at least in part. For example, Zhang et al (2002) generated a
mutant mouse in which c-fos expression was largely eliminated in the
hippocampus. They found that these mutant mice displayed more severe
kainic acid-induced seizures and increased neuronal excitability and neuronal
cell death. In vivo and in vitro experiment showed that c-fos regulated the
expression of the kainic acid receptor GluR6 and brain-derived neurotophic
factor (BDNF), indicating that c-fos played a role in regulating cellular
mechanisms that mediate neuronal excitability and survival.
Moreover, the expression of c-fos and its protein product, c-Fos, is used as an
index of neuronal excitation. As such, c-fos is the most widely used functional
anatomical marker of activated neurons. There are several features that render
this IEG as an excellent mapping tool: 1) it is expressed at low levels under
basal conditions; 2) the response is transient; 3) it is induced upon wide range
of extracellular stimuli (Kovacs, 1998, 2008). Indeed, multiple cis-acting
37
elements (e.g. Ca2+/CRE, SRE, SIS and AP-1RE) are present in the 5’flanking
regulatory region of the c-fos gene. Thus, c-fos is induced in response to
neurotransmitters and polypeptide hormones via cAMP response element
(CRE) in a process that involves activation of protein kinase A (PKA) or
calmodulin-independent protein kinases (CaMKs) which in turn phosphorylate
cAMP-responsive element-binding protein (CREB); the phosphorylated
CREB then binds to CRE and subsequently activates transcription (Gonzalez
and Montminy, 1989; Bito et al., 1996).
Whereas, c-fos expression is induced via serum response element (SRE) by
growth factors, cytokines, calcium and other stimuli through the action of
mitogen activated protein kinase (MAPK; Treisman, 1992). The sis-inducible
element (SIE) has been proposed to contribute to c-fos induction by plateletderived growth factor (PDGF) and gamma-interferon (Wagner et al., 1990).
The activator protein-1 response element (AP-1RE) is believed to be the site
where AP-1 might mediate negative auto-feedback for c-Fos transcription
(Sassone-Corsi et al., 1988).
At the protein level, c-Fos, product of c-fos, is a member of a family that
includes three other protein members, namely, FosB, Fra-1 and Fra-2
(Greenberg and Ziff, 1984; Cohen and Curran, 1988; Zerial et al., 1989;
Nishina et al., 1990). These proteins possess a leucin zipper motif that forms
heterodimers with Jun family proteins, and the resulting activator protein-1
(AP-1) complexes regulates subsequent gene expression by binding to the AP1 sequence found in many cellular genes (Chiu et al., 1988; Halazonetis et al.,
38
1988; Kouzarides and Ziff, 1988). The heterodimer AP-1 complexes interact
with a consensus sequence TGACTCA in the regulatory regions of target
genes and, depending on which member of the Jun family Fos dimerize with,
stimulate or repress transcription (Kovacs, 1998). For example, c-Fos/c-Jun
complexes were shown to exert stimulatory effects on target gene expression,
whereas c-Fos/JunB complexes were mostly inhibitory (Schutte et al., 1989).
1.4.2 Fos as an index of spinal nociceptive information processing
The expression of c-Fos in spinal cord following noxious stimulation was
initially reported by Hunt et al. (1987). As such it is a much used as a way to
visualize the spinal neurons involved in integration of noxious input. Indeed,
c-Fos is induced robustly following nociceptive stimuli in nuclei of spinal
neurons that are localized in the laminae I-II, the deep dorsal horn and the
ventral gray which is consistent with the distribution of nociceptive neurons
recorded using electrophysiological techniques (Millan, 1999; Coggeshall,
2005).
Whereas, non-noxious stimuli used as controls are generally
ineffective in expressing c-Fos in the neuronal population within these spinal
laminae.
A variety of noxious stimuli induce c-Fos expression in the spinal dorsal horn.
The most used noxious somatic stimuli include thermal (focused hot spots,
dipping the lower extremity into hot water, the hot plate test, etc), mechanical
(pinch, incision, pinprick), chemical (intraplantar injection of formalin and
39
carageenan, cutaneous injection of mustard or turpentine oils, etc.; Coggeshall,
2005).
Interestingly, the spatial pattern of c-Fos expression in spinal cord may vary
with time after peripheral application of various noxious stimuli including
thermal (Herdegen et al., 1991; Schadrack et al., 1998), mechanical (Leah et
al., 1992) and chemicals such as formalin (Presley et al., 1990a; Li et al.,
2004), carrageenan (Draisci and Iadarola, 1989) and mustard oil (Hunt et al.,
1987). For example, Presley et al (1990) reported that following injection of
formalin (5%, 150µl), robust c-Fos expression was detected at 1 hr in laminae
I and II after injection. Numbers of c-Fos neuronal profiles rose to a maximum
in superficial laminae within 2-4 hr, and later declined and returned to basal
levels 8-24 hr following stimulation. At 2 hr after injection, labeling was also
observed in laminae III-IV, the deep dorsal horn (laminae V-VI) and in the
ventral gray (laminae VII, VIII and X). A parsimonious interpretation of the
pattern of temporal change is that the first wave of c-Fos induction in the
superficial laminae partly represents direct activation of spinal neurons
receiving nociceptive input while the second wave of c-Fos induction reflects
transsynaptic changes. Indeed, in context of the former, injection of formalin
induced c-Fos expression in ~80% of supraspinally projecting and neurokinin
1 (NK1) receptor-positive lamina 1 neurons, many of which received synapses
from substance P containing, presumably nociceptive primary afferents (Naim
et al., 1997; Todd et al., 2002). Conversely, deletion of NK1 receptor
significantly reduced formalin-induced nociceptive behaviour that was
accompanied by a decrease in number of c-Fos positive neurons in laminae III of the spinal cord (DeFelipe and Gonzalez-Albo, 1998)
40
Khanna et al (2004) reported that formalin-induced increase in spinal c-Fos
was formalin concentration-dependent and paralleled the increase in
nociceptive behavior. The concentration-dependent increase in c-Fos
expression was observed in all laminae, especially in laminae I-II, V-VI and
VII-X. Conversely, c-Fos expression decreased in concert with nociceptive
behaviors in the formalin test with the following manipulations: 1) local
plantar injection of alpha-2 adrenergic receptor agonist, medetomidine
(Pertovaara et al., 1993); 2) systemic or local plantar injection of opiates,
including morphine (Presley et al., 1990b; Barr et al., 2003); 3) fear
conditioning (Abbadie et al., 1994); 4) pre-treatment of capsaicin that
destroyed C-fibers, presumably including C-fiber nociceptors (Borsani et al.,
2007). Put together, the above-mentioned studies suggests that spinal c-Fos
expression following formalin injection is well correlated with nociceptive
behaviours and can be used as a marker of nociception.
1.5 AMPA
RS-α-Amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazole propionic acid (AMPA) is a
specific agonist for the AMPA receptor where it mimics the effects of the
neurotransmitter glutamate (Purves et al., 2008). The neurotoxic effect of
AMPA in the supramammillary region was first reported by Pan and
MacNaughton (2002). They found that AMPA was able to produce specific
lesions within a small region such as mSuM. More importantly, there was no
lesion in any rat extended into the mammillary bodies (Pan and MacNaughton,
2002). Therefore, AMPA was chosen in the current study to damage SuM
41
while sparing the adjacent mammillary bodies. As such, it rules out the
possibility that the behavioural effects of SuM lesions made by AMPA
resulted from lesions of the mammillary bodies.
1.6 Rationale and objectives of the present study
There are physiological (Khanna and Sinclair, 1989; Khanna, 1997; Khanna et
al., 2004), pharmacological (Rodgers and Brown, 1976) and behavioral
(McKenna and Melzack, 2001) data which suggest that the hippocampal
formation is recruited by noxious stimuli and contributes to the negative
affective-motional state of the animal during pain. Indeed, inactivation of the
hippocampus decreased formalin-pain induced behaviors in rat (McKenna and
Melzack, 2001).
The posterior hypothalamus (PH)-supramammillary (SuM) region is
reciprocally connected to the hippocampal formation. Furthermore, the region
modulates hippocampal plasticity and neuronal synchronization, in form of
hippocampal theta wave activity, through the medial septum (Ariffin et al.;
Jiang and Khanna, 2004; Pan and McNaughton, 2004; Jiang and Khanna,
2006). Interestingly, the behavioral effects of SuM lesion overlap with the
effects reported with lesion of the septum and the hippocampus (Pan and
McNaughton, 2002, 2004). This strengthens the postulate that the SuM
42
interaction with the septohippocampal region is important for affectivemotivational behaviors.
Most of investigations with SuM lesion have focused to date on the role of
region in anxiety, and learning and memory (e.g. Pan and McNaughton, 2002;
See review by Pan and McNaughton, 2004). Whereas, a recent report from the
laboratory indicated that SuM also acted as relay of nociceptive information to
the hippocampus (Ariffin et al., 2010). To investigate whether SuM affects
nociception, the present study evaluated the effects of PH-SuM lesion on
formalin-induced behaviours in rat. Given the binding of the PH-SuM region
with the septohippocampal region, it was anticipated that PH-SuM lesion
would attenuate formalin-induced nociception.
43
MATERIALS AND METHODS
44
2.1 Animals
42 adult male Sprague-Dawley rats weighing 270-300g at the time of surgery
were used. Following the surgery, the animals were housed individually in
polycarbonate cages and maintained on a 12-h light/dark cycle (on at 0700h,
off at 1900 h). Water and food were available ad libitum. All efforts were
made to minimize animal suffering. The current study has been approved by
Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC).
2.2 Surgery procedure
Rats were first injected atropine sulphate (2mg/kg, Sigma, St louis, USA) i.p.
to reduce bronchial and salivary secretions for improving respiration during
surgery. After 10min, the rats were anaesthetized by i.p. injection of sodium
pentobarbitone (60mg/kg; Abbot Laboratories, Australia). The head was then
secured in a stereotaxic apparatus (Stereotaxic frame assembly, Schueler,
USA). An incision was made into the scalp to expose the skull. The position of
the incisor bar was then adjusted so that bregma and lambda were in the same
horizontal plane. A hole was drilled into the skull for microinjection of 0.3 ml
of 0.015M RS-α-Amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazole propionic acid
(AMPA; Tocris Cookson Inc., USA) made up in 0.01M phosphate buffer (pH
7.4) at the rate of 0.1 ml/min via a 33G stainless steel needle. Vehicle animals
received similar injections of the phosphate buffer. The co-ordinates were AP
0.38, ML 0.07 and DV 0.80 mm (at an angle of 7° from the vertical) for
medial SuM and AP 0.38, ML 0.07 and DV 0.82 for lateral SuM. The needle
45
was left in situ for 10 minutes before retraction to prevent back flow of the
liquid and to allow diffusion into the surrounding tissue area. Nylon thread
was used to stitch up the incision wound. They were treated with diazepam
(2.5 mg/kg i.p; Mayne Pharma Pty Ltd; Australia) following surgery so as to
control seizures that may otherwise be observed due to microinjection of
AMPA. The rats were allowed to recover for about 10 days in animal holding
unit (AHU) in National University of Singapore before behavioural tests.
2.3 Behavioural tests
2.3.1 Scheduling of the behavioural tests
15 animals were tested only in open field after which they were sacrificed.
Another 27 animals that were also tested in open field were subsequently
subject to the formalin test of persistent inflammatory pain. The formalin test
was performed on the 4th day following the open field experiment. In the
intervening period the animals were habituated to the experimental apparatus
for at least 45 min every day. The animals were sacrificed after formalin test.
2.3.2 Open field test
A 43.2 x 43.2 x 30.5cm (L X WQ X H) Open Field (OF) Activity chamber
with transparent walls (OFA, Med Associates Inc., USA) was used for the
open field test. The apparatus was elevated 70 cm above the floor.
46
Environmental temperature was kept at 22±1˚C. The animal was transferred
from the AHU using a 30cm x 20cm clean plastic carrier box to the laboratory
located within 10 min walking time. The animal was placed at the right lower
corner of the chamber immediately after the observer reached laboratory and
allowed to explore for 90 min in the absence of the observer (Fig 4).
The OF apparatus was equipped with infrared (I/R) photo beams aimed at I/R
detectors that were used to detect animal movement along the horizontal (# I/R
beams) and vertical (# I/R beams) directions. Upon entry of the animal, signals
from the I/R beams were fed into a computer and the number of IR beams
broken by the animal provided measures of ambulation distance, velocity,
vertical counts, stereotypic count and etc.
The exploratory behaviours were scored as the ambulatory distance which was
obtained from the computer system. An ambulatory episode was signalled
when the rat moved beyond 4 IR beams (i.e. Box Size) in 500 ms (i.e. Resting
Delay).
After 90 min, the animal was moved to the carrier box brought into the
behavioural room and habituated in that for another 30 min. Food and water
were available to the animal in that period. Subsequently, the animals were
sent back to AHU for formalin test 4 days later.
2.3.3 Formalin test
47
Open Field Apparatus
I/R dischargers
Computer
Signal processing
system
I/R detectors
Figure 4: Open field apparutus. The Open Field Activity (OFA) chamber
(43.2 X 43.2 X 30.5 cm) was used. The photo beams from the I/R dischargers
were received by the I/R detectors along both the horizontal and vertical
directions. Upon entry of the animals, signals from the I/R beams were fed
into a computer and the number of I/R beams broken by an animal provided
measure of ambulation distance.
48
One half (21.7 x 43.2 x 30.5 cm) of the ‘two-compartment insert’ (Med
Associates Inc., USA) was used for formalin test (Fig 5). The insert was
placed inside the OFA and the half selected for the test had grid flooring. The
formalin test involved injecting 0.1ml of dilute formalin (1.25%; made from
37% formaldehyde, Merck) subcutaneously into the plantar surface of the
right hind paw of the rat with a 27G needle (Becton Dickinson & Co.).The
animal was restrained manually during the hind paw injection. Following the
injection, the animal was placed in the selected half of the ‘two-compartment
insert’.
The nociceptive behaviours were scored for 90min starting immediately after
the injection of formalin. Parameters such as number of flinches and duration
of licks were recorded manually by the observer while ambulatory distance
was recorded by the computer. Flinching comprises shaking of the injected
hindpaw, flinching of the hindquarters and the reflexive withdrawal of the
injected hind paw. Each occurrence of shaking, flinching of hindquarters and
reflexive withdrawal of the paw was considered as one count of flinching. The
duration of licks was recorded as the time of licking of both the dorsal plantar
surface of the injected paw. Biting of the nails was not included in scoring the
nociceptive behaviour.
After 90 min, the animal was placed back and habituated in the carrier box
with water and food in the behavioural room for another 30 min. 2 hours after
the injection of formalin, the animal was perfused (see section 2.4.1) and the
spinal cord take out for c-Fos immunocytochemistry.
49
Formalin Test Apparatus
I/R dischargers
Computer
Signal processing
system
I/R detectors
Figure 5: Formalin test apparatus. For the purpose of the formalin test a
two-compartment insert was placed into the Open Field Activity chamber. The
two compartments of the insert were of similar size (21.7 X 43.2 X 30.5 cm
each) but each had different flooring, namely grid or rod flooring. The rat was
tested in the half with the grid flooring.
50
2.4 Immunocytochemistry
2.4.1 Tissue preparation
The animal was deeply anaesthetized with urethane (1.5g/kg, i.p; Sigma, St
Louis, MO, USA). Heparin (0.1ml, 1000IU/ml; Sigma, St Louis, MO, USA)
was injected into the left ventricle of the heart followed by transcardial
perfusion of 400ml sodium nitrite solution (1%, Merck, Germany), and later of
400ml of paraformaldehyde solution (4%, Merck, Germany). The two
solutions were made in 0.5M and 0.1M sodium phosphate (Merck, Germany)
buffer, respectively.
The brain and the spinal cord were removed, blocked, post fixed in the above
fixative for approximately 24 h and 4 hr at 4 ˚C, respectively. Alternate
coronal 60 µm sections were taken through the brain and the lumbar spinal
cord on a microtome with vibrating blade (Leica, Germany) and collected in
0.05M Tris-buffered saline (Fisher Scientific).
2.4.2 Staining procedures
Previously described method (Zheng and Khanna, 2001; Khanna et al., 2004)
were adapted for OX42 labeling and Fos immunocytochemistry of the brain
sections. Alternate sections of the L4 lumbar spinal cord and the brain sections
taken through the hippocampus were immunolabeled for c-Fos. In addition,
alternate brain sections through the SuM were immunolabeled for OX42. The
51
Avidin-biotin-peroxidase complex (ABC) technique was applied to detect the
antigen.
Anitibody OX42 recognizes the rat equivalent of human CD11b, the receptor
for the iC3b component of complement. The antigen is expressed on most
macrophages, including resident and activated peritoneal macrophages and
Kupffer cells and around 35% of alveolar macrophages. The antibody also
labels dendritic cells, granulocytes and microglial cells in the brain (Whiteland
et al., 1995).
All procedures were carried out at 4 ˚C unless otherwise specified. Sections
were incubated in wells of a 24-well cell culture plate (1 section/well, 300µl
/well for brain; 2 sections/well, 250µl/well for spinal cord). Briefly, the
sections were rinsed with 0.3% hydrogen peroxide to quench endogenous
peroxidase for 20 min, and were incubated for 2 h in room temperature in 3%
bovine serumalbumin (Sigma, St Louis, MO, USA) in 0.05 M Tris-buffered
saline with 0.3%Triton X-100 (Bio-Rad Labs, USA) to block non-specific
protein interactions. Subsequently, the sections were incubated for 70 h with
the primary antibody (1:1000 mouse anti-rat anti-OX42 monoclonal antibody,
AbD Serotec, Germany or 1:2000 rabbit anti-Fos (Ab-5) polyclonal antibody,
Calbiochem, Germany espectively), followed by 3 h incubation with the
secondary antibody (1:1000 biotinylated anti-mouse antibody for OX42,
Vector Labs,USA or 1:1000 biotinylated goat anti-rabbit antibody for c-Fos,
Calbiochem, Germany, respectively). All antibodies were diluted in 0.05M
TBS containing BSA and 0.3% Trition® X-100. Sections were then treated
52
with the ABC (Vectastain Elite ABC Kit, Vector Labs, USA) for 3 h with
constant shaking at room temperature on an orbital shaker (N-biotek, Korea)
followed by diaminobenzidine treatment (DAB in 0.05% in 0.05 MTBS;
Sigma, USA). Once the brown immunolabel had developed, the reaction was
stopped. The sections were mounted on chrome alum gelatine-coated slides,
air dried, dehydrated via ethanol (BDH Laboratory Supplies, UK) and ethanolxylene (J.T.Baker, Canada) cleared with xylene and coverslipped with DePeX
(BDH Laboratory Supplies, UK). The Fos-like immunoreactivity (FLI) was
visualised as brown reaction product.
2.5 Data analysis
2.5.1 Quantification of the size of the lesion in the brain
Sections through the SuM region in the brain were digitized at 500 dpi using
Montage Explorer (Synoptics Ltd.) using a Nikon Eclipse E408 microscope
(Nikon Corp., Japan) at 2x. The lesion area in each section was clearly
observed as dark brown cluster of activated microglia that was immunostained
with OX-42 antibody. The quantification of the lesion size was done using
commercially available software (AISTM Image Analysis Software; Imaging
Research Inc., Canada). Briefly, the outer margin of the immunolabeled region
was taken as the boundary of the lesioned area (see Fig 9). The software was
programmed to detect and highlight the demarcated region which was at least
three times more intense than the background. The condition was set based on
comparisons with manual selections of the lesioned area. Subsequently, the
53
software calculated the area of the highlighted region. Given that the thickness
of the section was 0.06 mm, the volume of the lesion region was obtained by
multiplying the area with 0.06 for each section.
2.5.2 Quantification of Fos positive cells in spinal cord and hippocampus
Alternate sections of the lumbar L4 spinal cord were digitized at 500dpi using
Montage Explorer (Synoptics Ltd.) using a Nikon Eclipse E408 microscope
(Nikon Corp., Japan) at 4x and counted using a commercially available
software (AISTM Image Analysis Software; Imaging Research Inc., Canada).
Briefly, the average intensity of each spinal region was determined and Fospositive cells in that region were detected as stained nuclei that were 300%
intense as compared with the average intensity of the region (Lee et al., 2008).
Besides the intensity difference, another two criteria were applied in
identifying c-Fos positive neurons: 1) an area of greater than or equal to 5
units, and 2) a form factor of greater than 0.8. The form factor is a measure of
the degree of roundness and a form factor of 1 is perfectly round.
The c-Fos positive cells were counted for the spinal cord on the right side
(ipsilateral to the formalin injection) of the lumbar L4 spinal segments.
Laminar specific counts were made for four regions as described (Khanna et
al., 2004). The spinal segmental level and the laminar organization were
identified based on the configuration of the gray matter (Molander et al., 1984).
The regions with corresponding laminae were: (A) superficial dorsal horn
(laminae I-II), (B) nucleus proprius (laminae III-IV), (C) neck of the dorsal
54
horn (laminae V-VI) and (D) the ventral gray (laminae VII-X) (Fig 6). In
addition, the total count was averaged for all L4 sections of each animal and
then for the experimental group. The number of spinal sections through L4
that were analyzed for FLI cells ranged from 15-20.
55
Figure 6. Diagrammatic representation of lumbar L4 spinal cord coronal
section with laminar subdivisions. (A) Superficial dorsal horn (laminae I-II);
(B) nucleus proprius (laminae III-IV); (C) neck of the dorsal horn (laminae VVI); (D) ventral gray (laminae VII-X). (Adapted from Molander et al., 1984;
Khanna et al., 2004)
56
The hippocampal c-Fos-positive cells counted manually using an Olympus
microscope at 40x. A grid was inserted into the eyepiece that facilitated
counting in non-overlapping squares over the region of interest. The cells were
counted bilaterally in the pyramidal cell layer of CA1 and CA3 and dentate
granule cell layer from alternate sections taken through the anterior-posterior
extent of the hippocampus corresponding to bregma -1.72 to bregma -5.76 mm
(Paxinos and Watson, 2007). Generally, 30-35 hippocampal sections were
analysed per animal. As with spinal cord, the total count for each hippocampal
region was averaged for the sections of each animal, and then for the
experimental group. Such averages were also calculated for dorsal and ventral
CA1. The dorsal CA1 included the anterior region of CA1 (bregma -1.72 to
bregma -4.65 mm; Fig.7) and the dorsal CA1 in the posterior sections of the
hippocampus (bregma -4.65 to bregma -5.76 mm; Fig.8). The dorsal CA1 in
posterior sections was demarcated from ventral CA1 by the intervening CA2.
2.5.3 Statistical Analysis
The results are expressed as mean ± S.E.M. One-way ANOVA with post hoc
Newman-Keul’s test for multiple comparisons was performed for statistical
comparisons of data from more than two groups. Comparison of data between
two groups was performed using two-tailed unpaired t-test. Statistical
significance was accepted at p[...]... population spike suppression In addition, there may be functional and neurochemical heterogeneity along the mediolateral axis of SuM neurons in controlling different aspects of hippocampal theta such as frequency and amplitude 1. 2.4 Effects of manipulations of SuM on behaviour and hippocampal theta The effect of manipulations of SuM has been examined on a variety of animal behaviors Especially the focus of. .. contributes to the negative affective-motivational state during pain In the present study we explored whether PH-SuM as well modulate animal pain behaviors in the formalin model of persistent inflammatory pain Formalin (1. 25%, 0.1ml) was injected subcutaneously into the plantar surface of the right hind paw of PH-SuM lesioned or control nonlesioned animals The lesion was induced by microinjection of. .. al., 19 76; Ottersen, 19 80; Swanson, 19 82; Haglund et al., 19 84; Gonzalo-Ruiz et al., 19 92b; Hayakawa and Zyo, 19 94; Leranth and Kiss, 19 96; 15 Borhegyi et al., 19 98; Vertes and McKenna, 2000; Kiss et al., 2002) In addition, the projections from SuM and to SuM are chemically very diverse In the following section, more focus will be given to connections pertinent to hippocampus and septal area 1. 2 .1 Projections... periolivary region Shaffer collateral sis-inducible element supramammillary Lateral supramammillary transient receptor potential family of ion channels wide dynamic range wheat germ agglutinin conjugated to horseradish peroxidase x LIST OF PUBLICATIONS ABSTRACT Liu LM and Khanna S (2009) Effects of posterior hypothalamic lesions on formalin- induced pain behaviours Proceedings of the International Australasian... Buzsaki, 19 96) 1. 1.2 Intrinsic connection within the hippocampus formation A unidirectional intrinsic connection which consists of a series of excitatory pathways enables a sequential flow of information through the hippocampal 6 formation It is organized in a trisynaptic circuit, starting from the perforant pathway from the entorhinal cortex to the dentate gyrus, continues with the mossy fibres projection... thalamus, posterior hypothalamus, amygdale, medial prefrontal and the entorhinal cortices (Swanson, 19 82; Shibata, 19 87; Vertes, 19 88; Hayakawa et al., 19 93; Risold and Swanson, 19 97) 1. 2.2 Projections to SuM Injections of wheat germ agglutinin conjugated to horseradish peroxidase (WGA-HRP) into the supramammillary nucleus resulted in retrogradely labelled neurons in MS-VLDBB region and other regions including... Glutamate stimulation of SuML produced a substantial increase in the perforant path stimulation -induced population spike in the dentate gyrus (Carre and Harley, 19 91) Inactivation of the mSuM with GABA abolished reticularly-elicited suppression of CA1 population spike, thus indicating that SuM mediated reticularly-elicited suppression of CA1 pyramidal cell synaptic excitability to CA3 stimulation (Jiang and... regulation of entorhinal synapses terminating on the distal dendrites of the pyramidal cells (Blasco-Ibanez and Freund, 19 95; Katona et al., 19 99) 10 1. 1.3 Extrinsic connection of hippocampus formation Two types of inputs to hippocampal principle cells have been identified: 1) laminated afferents from cortical areas (such as entorhinal area and amygdala) terminating in the specific layers along the... Projections from SUM SuM as a whole contains calretinin-containing cells (Kiss et al., 2000), dopaminergic cells (Swanson, 19 82), and substance P containing cells (Borhegyi and Leranth, 19 97; Fig 3) The most medial portion of SuM (mSuM) contains small packed cells and is distinguished by having substance P containing cells Cells in this region have weak connections to hippocampus and these connections... Antal, 19 88) Glutamatergic neurons concentrate in the medial septal regions and are distributed among the cholinergic and GABAergic neurons (Colom et al., 2005) 1. 1.3.2 Hippocampal-septal projection The hippocampal-septal projection is the most prominent efferent projections from the hippocampus among all the efferent projections from the region (Leranth et al., 19 92) The main subcortical target of this ... connections 11 1. 1.3.2 Hippocampal-septal projection 12 ii 1. 1.4 Hippocampal theta rhythm 1. 2 Anatomy of supramammillary area (SuM) 13 15 1. 2 .1 Projections from SuM 16 1. 2.2 Projections to SuM 18 1. 2.3... schedule 22 1. 3 Pain 24 1. 3 .1 General description of pain 24 1. 3.2 Pain transmission pathway 27 1. 3.3 Formalin model of persistent pain 32 1. 4 c-Fos 37 1. 4 .1 General description of c-Fos 37 1. 4.2 Fos... gyrus 1. 1.2.2 The mossy fibres projection from the dentate gyrus to CA3 1. 1.2.3 The Schaffer collaterals to CA1 1. 1.3 Extrinsic connection of hippocampus formation 11 1. 1.3 .1 Septo-hippocampal connections