1. Trang chủ
  2. » Giáo án - Bài giảng

auditory temporal processing deficit in dyslexia is associated with enhanced sensitivity in the visual modality

4 0 0

Đang tải... (xem toàn văn)

THÔNG TIN TÀI LIỆU

NEUROREPORT COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE Auditory temporal processing de®cit in dyslexia is associated with enhanced sensitivity in the visual modality Sabine Heim,CA Robert B Freeman Jr, Carsten Eulitz and Thomas Elbert Department of Psychology, University of Konstanz, P.O Box D25, 78457 Konstanz, Germany CA Corresponding Author Received 20 November 2000; accepted December 2000 Developmental dyslexia has been associated with a de®cit in temporal processing, but it is controversial whether the postulated de®cit is pansensory or limited to the auditory modality We present psychophysical assessment data of auditory and visual temporal processing abilities in children with dyslexia While none of the dyslexic children displayed temporal processing abnormalities in the visual sensory mod- ality, dyslexics with poor auditory temporal scores reached high-level visual performance Our results not con®rm the hypothesis of a general temporal processing de®cit for dyslexia but suggest that limitations in auditory temporal processing might be compensated for by a well-functioning visual sensory modality NeuroReport 12:507±510 & 2001 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins Key words: Auditory; Children; Dyslexia; Temporal processing; Visual INTRODUCTION Developmental dyslexia is characterized by a failure in learning to read, spell and write despite normal intellectual capacity, educational resources, and adequate sociocultural opportunity, even though sensory de®cits, neurological pathology, and other impediments to attaining literacy skills have been ruled out [1,2] Psychoacoustic studies have shown that many children with language and literacy problems display limitations in reception of brief and rapidly changing phonemes (e.g stop consonant syllables) or rapidly successive sound inputs [3,4] for which Paula Tallal has been using the term `temporal processing de®cit' Experimental ®ndings in the visual and tactile sensory modalities led some researchers to conclude that a temporal processing de®cit might be pansensory in children with language impairment and people with dyslexia [3,4] Because multimodal approaches within a single study have been sparse and inconclusive, the question of whether a temporal processing de®cit in these populations might be general or modality speci®c has been a subject of intense debate The present study aimed to investigate temporal processing abilities in both the auditory and visual modality in children with dyslexia and normally literate controls The auditory task required a same±different judgment of two successively presented stop consonant±vowel (CV) syllables (/ba/ and /da/), which are characterized by rapid frequency changes (formant transitions) that occur during the initial few tens of milliseconds As a control condition, the same syllables were presented with temporally ex- 0959-4965 & Lippincott Williams & Wilkins tended formant transitions [5] This control procedure should provide an evaluation of the auditory temporal processing hypothesis for dyslexia: a relative weakness in discriminating between CV syllables with rapidly changing formant transitions compared with CV syllables with temporally extended formant transitions would support the view of a de®cit in temporal acoustic processing; poor performance on both conditions would suggest that the dif®culties experienced by the dyslexic children are speech-speci®c in general In accordance with Tallal's work, performance on the two conditions was measured at different interstimulus intervals (ISIs) varying between and 305 ms [5,6] In the visual task, thresholds for the detection of temporal order of two light ¯ashes were determined The ®ndings of the study should add valuable information to the issue of whether a temporal processing de®cit in dyslexia is pansensory or modality speci®c MATERIALS AND METHODS We tested 22 children (three females) diagnosed with developmental dyslexia and 11 controls (one female) with normal literacy skills who matched the others in mother tongue (all native speakers of German), age and non-verbal intelligence (Table 1) Sixteen children from the dyslexic group attended a special school for dyslexia, the other six having been referred by an independent institute that specializes in diagnosing and treating children with learning disabilities Although the dyslexic subjects were receiving remedial training and intense tutoring, they performed signi®cantly worse on measures of reading, phonological Vol 12 No March 2001 507 NEUROREPORT S HEIM Table Psychometric data for study groups: means Ỉ s.d Controls (n ˆ 11) Age (years) Range Non-verbal IQ Standard reading Errors (z-scores) Time (s) Pseudoword reading % errors Standard spelling Errors (z-scores) Dyslexics (n ˆ 22) t-test 13.6 Ỉ 1.6 11.1±15.4 106.4 Ỉ 14.0 13.2 Ỉ 1.4 11.0±15.8 108.6 Ỉ 11.0 n.s À0.9 Ỉ 0.4 125.0 Æ 12.4 0.4 Æ 0.9 184.2 Æ 41.6 p , 0.000003a p , 0.000006b 5.0 Ỉ 2.5 16.0 Ỉ 5.5 p , 0.000000a À1.2 Ỉ 0.6 0.6 Ỉ 0.5 p , 0.000000 n.s a t-test for unequal variances Statistical comparison is based on logarithmic data n.s., not signi®cant ( p 0.05) Non-verbal intelligence was assessed with Raven's Standard Progressive Matrices [14] Reading and phonological decoding skills were examined using the ZuÈrcher Lesetest [15] and a non-standardized pseudoword reading test (40 items), respectively Depending on the child's grade, the Diagnostische Rechtschreibtest [16] or Westermann Rechtschreibtest [17] was administered to evaluate spelling abilities Due to a lack of German normative data for older children, we decided not to transform reading and spelling raw scores into % ranks; z-scores (mean ˆ 0, s.d ˆ 1) are therefore given b decoding and spelling than the controls (Table 1) All subjects had normal hearing thresholds and normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity Informed consent was obtained from the parents of the children All children were rewarded with shopping vouchers or cinema tickets for their voluntary participation The psychophysical tasks were carried out on an IBM compatible 486 PC The order of the tasks was counterbalanced across subjects Each child was tested individually in a quiet room Auditory same±different task: The German CV syllables /ba/ and /da/ were created with a sampling rate of 10 kHz in a cascade mode by using Speechlab software [7] based on a Klatt cascade/parallel formant synthesizer [8] The total stimulus duration was 250 ms including a formant transition (FT) period of either 40 ms (rapid FT condition) or 90 ms (extended FT condition) The fundamental frequency of each syllable started at 128 Hz and decreased linearly to 109 Hz at stimulus offset The syllables were composed of three formants (F1±F3) and differed in the onset frequencies of the second and third FT The starting points of the second and third FT were 1095 Hz and 2100 Hz for the syllable /ba/ and 1702 Hz and 2633 Hz for /da/ The steady-state formant frequencies of the vowel /a/ were 770, 1340, and 2400 Hz for F1, F2, and F3, respectively The amplitude of voicing was constant at 54 dB and fell linearly to 11 dB during the last 25 ms of the stimulus The experimental procedure was identical for the rapid and extended FT conditions The syllables were delivered to both ears via Sony MDR-CD470 headphones at $72 dB sound pressure level The task required the child to press the right (green) panel if two successive syllables (/ba/± /ba/, /da/±/da/) were the same and the left (red) panel if they were different (/ba/±/da/, /da/±ba/) by using the index ®nger of her/his right hand Feedback was provided after each stimulus pair (trial) by a happy or unhappy face on the computer screen The intertrial interval was s The training phase consisted of a maximum of 508 Vol 12 No March 2001 72 trials (18 for each possible pair combination, randomly intermixed) and ®nished earliest if a criterion of 30 correct responses in 36 consecutive trials was achieved The task was terminated for subjects who did not meet the criterion During training an ISI (de®ned as syllable offset to onset) of 428 ms was employed; in the immediately following testing phase syllables were presented at six different ISIs: 8, 15, 30, 60, 150 and 305 ms [5,6] Testing included 48 trials, eight (2 syllable pair combinations) for each ISI, with a randomized presentation of the different intervals The percentages of correct trials were measured at each ISI Visual temporal order task: Equiluminant light ¯ashes of green and red were generated by two light-emitting diodes (LEDs) LEDs were mounted side by side (distance cm) on a black surface slanted toward the subject The LED apparatus was positioned on a table ($70 cm high) with a constant viewing distance of 40 cm Subjects were asked to press either a red (left) or green (right) key on the computer keyboard with the index ®nger of their right hand indicating the LED which ¯ashed ®rst After the second LED ¯ashed, both LEDs remained on for s Twoelement stimulus sequences (red±green, green±red) were randomized across trials The intertrial interval was s In order to direct subject's attention to the LEDs, each trial was announced by a brief tone During an initial training phase of 10 trials, onsets between two light ¯ashes (SOA, stimulus onset asynchrony) varied between 310 and 400 ms, i.e rather long intervals, to allow all subjects to understand the task In the testing phase (40 trials) the SOA was adjusted from trial to trial (starting SOA ˆ 300 ms), using an adaptation of the staircase procedure [9] After one correct response in a given staircase, the SOA was shortened, whereas an incorrect answer led to the SOA being lengthened Sizes of downward or upward steps (reversals) were 10% of the previous value; below a 10 ms SOA a step size of ms was used The visual temporal-order threshold in milliseconds, de®ned as the arithmetic mean of the last 20 reversals, was calculated automatically by the computer program NEUROREPORT TEMPORAL PROCESSING IN CHILDREN WITH DYSLEXIA RESULTS Auditory task Auditory task 100 90 % correct 80 extended FT rapid FT 70 60 50 305 150 60 30 15 305 150 60 30 15 ISI (ms) Control subjects (n ϭ 11) Dyslexic subjects (n ϭ 22) Fig Percentage correct for 11 control (open circles) and 22 dyslexic (®lled triangles) subjects on the auditory same±different task at various ISIs of the rapid and extended FT conditions 100 90 80 % correct Univariate ANOVA showed no signi®cant group effect on visual temporal-order thresholds, indicating that both the dyslexic (mean (Ỉ s.e.m.) threshold 8.9 Ỉ 1.2 ms) and control children (threshold 9.9 Ỉ 1.3 ms) managed the visual temporal processing task without dif®culty and equally well Mixed-design ANOVA performed on the data of auditory processing revealed a signi®cant group main effect (F(1,31) ˆ 4.2, p , 0.050) for the dyslexics to have lower scores than the controls in both FT conditions across the range of ISIs tested (Fig 1) Even though no signi®cant interaction was obtained with the factor group there was a small tendency for larger group differences in the rapid than in the temporally extended FT condition As demonstrated by other investigators, some people with dyslexia may have no dif®culties in tasks involving rapidly changing acoustic stimuli [3,4] This might also apply to a subset of dyslexic children in the present study and in turn have weakened the group condition interaction Therefore, the dyslexic subjects were subclassi®ed into two groups based on their discrimination performance on the rapidly changing syllables in the three shortest ISIs (8, 15 and 30 ms) Dyslexic subjects scoring > 87.5% in two of the shortest ISIs were classi®ed as good perceivers, otherwise they were classi®ed as poor perceivers Mixed-design ANOVA carried out with these groups yielded a signi®cant group FT condition interaction effect (F(2,30) ˆ 6.8, p , 0.004) As illustrated in Fig 2, the 14 dyslexic subjects comprising the group of good perceivers did not differ from the controls in any of the conditions However, the poor- 70 60 50 Control subjects (n ϭ 11) Dyslexic subjects good perceiver (n ϭ 14) poor perceiver (n ϭ 8) extended FT rapid FT Fig Percentage correct on the auditory same±different task in the control subjects (open circles) and in dyslexic subjects who were subclassi®ed as good (open triangles) and poor (®lled triangles) perceivers according their performance in the three shortest ISIs of the rapid FT condition perceiver group (n ˆ dyslexic subjects) was signi®cantly less accurate in the rapid FT condition compared to the other groups (ScheffeÂ's p , 0.001) and their performance level in the extended FT condition (ScheffeÂ's p , 0.018) This result suggests that at least subjects of the dyslexic group displayed dif®culties in processing rapid temporal acoustic information In order to avoid possible ceiling effects, we used the minimum correct percentages of each subject attained at any ISI of the rapid FT condition for analyzing the relationship between auditory and visual temporal processing Individual subject data for minimum auditory scores in relation to visual temporal-order thresholds are presented in Fig A bivariate prediction analysis based on weighted Kappa indicated a signi®cant distribution pattern for the dyslexic group ( p , 0.004) Ten dyslexic subjects whose performance in the same±different task pointed to limitations in auditory temporal processing (i.e < 75% correct [10]), demonstrated low temporal-order thresholds (, 10 ms) in the visual sensory modality In ®ve of these children, visual thresholds were s.d below the group mean of normal controls In control subjects, the statistical relationship between measures on auditory and visual temporal processing did not reach signi®cance DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSION Our results reveal an auditory temporal processing de®cit in children with dyslexia, although temporal sensitivity was enhanced rather than impaired in the visual task Thus, the current data provide no evidence for a pansensory or general temporal processing de®cit in children with Vol 12 No March 2001 509 NEUROREPORT S HEIM by a study by Talcott et al [11] in a sample of unselected elementary school children: auditory and visual temporal processing were found to be differently engaged in phonological and orthographic skills, implying independent candidates in determining a child's ability to learn to read Our ®ndings support the view that intervention methods for dyslexia should target the auditory modality [12,13] Auditory temporal performance Min (% correct) 100 80 60 REFERENCES 40 Control subjects (n ϭ 11) Dyslexic subjects (n ϭ 22) 20 0 12 16 20 Visual temporal-order threshold (ms) 24 Fig Plot of auditory temporal performance in relation to visual temporal-order thresholds (ms) for 11 control (open circles) and 22 dyslexic (®lled triangles) children Min (% correct) denotes minimum correct percentages attained at any ISI of the rapid FT condition Low visual thresholds re¯ect superior performance dyslexia On the contrary, poor auditory temporal sensitivity might be compensated by a well functioning visual sensory modality Supportive evidence has been provided Miles T Dyslexia: The Pattern of Dif®culties Spring®eld, IL: Charles C Thomas, 1983 Dilling H, Mombour W and Schmidt MH International Classi®cation of Mental Diseases ICD-10 Bern: Huber, 1991 Tallal P, Miller S and Fitch RH Ann NY Acad Sci 682, 27±47 (1993) Farmer ME and Klein RM Psychon Bull Rev 2, 460±493 (1995) Tallal P and Piercy M Neuropsychologia 13, 69±74 (1975) Tallal P and Piercy M Neuropsychologia 12, 83±93 (1974) Diesch E Behav Res Methods Instr Comput 29, 302 (1997) Klatt DH J Acoust Soc Am 67, 971±995 (1980) Cornsweet TN Am J Psychol 75, 485±491 (1962) 10 Tallal P and Piercy M Nature 241, 468±469 (1973) 11 Talcott JB et al Proc Natl Acad Sci USA, 97, 2952±2957 (2000) 12 Merzenich MM et al Science 271, 77±81 (1996) 13 Tallal P et al Science 271, 81±84 (1996) 14 Heller KA, Kratzmeier H and Lengfelder A Standard Progressive Matrices von J C Raven GoÈttingen: Beltz, 1998 15 Linder M and Grissemann H ZuÈrcher Lesetest, ZLT Bern: Huber, 1998 16 Grund M, Haug G and Naumann CL Diagnostischer Rechtschreibtest fuÈr Klassen, DRT Weinheim: Beltz, 1995 17 Rathenow P, LaupenmuÈhlen D and VoÈge J Westermann Rechtschreibtest 6‡, WRT 6‡ Braunschweig: Westermann, 1981 Acknowledgements: We thank H Vlahu for help in data collection, W Nagl for advice on statistical analysis, P Dixon for important contributions towards programming the LEDs, and A Keil for comments on the manuscript This work was supported by grants from the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft 510 Vol 12 No March 2001 ... auditory and visual temporal processing Individual subject data for minimum auditory scores in relation to visual temporal- order thresholds are presented in Fig A bivariate prediction analysis... results reveal an auditory temporal processing de®cit in children with dyslexia, although temporal sensitivity was enhanced rather than impaired in the visual task Thus, the current data provide... temporal processing (i.e < 75% correct [10]), demonstrated low temporal- order thresholds (, 10 ms) in the visual sensory modality In ®ve of these children, visual thresholds were s.d below the group

Ngày đăng: 02/11/2022, 08:53

Xem thêm: