1. Trang chủ
  2. » Ngoại Ngữ

The-problem-with-researching-with-children.edited-CS-1

7 3 0

Đang tải... (xem toàn văn)

THÔNG TIN TÀI LIỆU

Nội dung

But what if they don’t ask the right questions? Problems of power and control in researching with children Carla Solvason* and Johanna Cliffe** *University of Worcester; **The Learning Institute, Cornwall In previous work we discussed ‘voice’ in collaborative research (Solvason, Cliffe and Snowden, 2017) looking at how ideas of power and ownership are negotiated and an authentic voice found Here we move into the even more complex arena of collaborative research with children Following the United Nations Rights of the Child (1989) adults woke up to the fact that children are entitled to present their own view about their lives; but we argue that despite a proliferation of research projects focusing on ‘the child’s voice’ (for example Bateman, 2017, Flückiger, Dunn and Stinson, 2018) we rarely see young children’s views at all Instead we see children's thoughts produced within prescribed parameters and filtered through the lens of adults Although we may get somewhere near listening to and understanding young children’s interpretations of their experience, the adult and child context remain worlds apart and ‘true’ meaning remains perspectival and lost in translation The problem To start with, most teachers in the primary years have a touch of the control freak – as primary teachers, we are allowed to say it We are not suggesting this is entirely bad, we are just used to keeping things (including children) on track, in line, ordered Disorder does not sit comfortably with us In our experience, early years practitioners (0-7) are used to ‘going with the flow’ a little more The reins can be looser whilst children make choices and explore But explore what? Explore an environment carefully planned by the practitioner ‘Choices’ children make are within restrictions that adults plan and offer Gallagher (2008: 25) explains that “child-adult relations are often characterised by domination and subordination.” That is simply how it is, adults make decisions and children comply He adds, “Even in the absence of coercion, children may rely upon cues from adults whom they trust” (2008: 16) Children are used to adults being in control Consider it this way, the adult arranges a trip to the beach, the child gets excited about the sea but would not have a clue how to get there (hot air balloon?) and certainly wouldn’t know to bring a towel To expect young children to step outside their subordinate role and take on the responsibility of ‘researcher’ is as feasible as allowing them to take the wheel and drive to the destination themselves They have neither knowledge nor experience of the necessary skills So James’ (2004: 158) suggestion that we abandon stereotypes of sage adult and inexperienced child and instead “see wisdom and uncertainty shared among people of varying ages and experience” is all very well, but who is responsible for collating said wisdom? Nonetheless, whilst child led research may be unfeasible with the very young, that should not prevent genuine endeavours to hear their unique perspective (Mukherji & Albon, 2010) This requires redressing power imbalances in early years settings to create an environment where ideas are not just listened to but genuinely valued The need for ethical and sensitive practice in research involving children goes without saying (Solvason 2016, 2017 and 2018) Care and respect should be evident and all processes diligently followed (British Educational Research Association, 2018) Here we move beyond ideas of consent or assent to seeing children become excited about making their own voyages of research discovery Knowledge and power Many are uncomfortable with the notion of classrooms as political spaces in the way knowledge and power subtly (sometimes not so subtly) operate, or to acknowledge that politics impacts on and controls children’s learning and their agency to act upon their world (Dahlberg and Moss 2005) Knowledge, in terms of our curricula is officially sanctioned and children are expected to re-tread and re-discover learning pathways that lead to what is already known (Cliffe and Solvason 2016) What begins with policy makers and becomes endorsed by educators is a comfortable credence to Platonian discourses of recognition Essentially we are encouraged to mould children’s thinking and ideas into versions of Sameness, under the guise that developing knowledge must equate to reaching developmental benchmarks and nationally set criteria Children are assessed, labelled and pigeonholed by the knowledge they possess, often overlooking the quality and nature of that knowledge in favour of the social and economic power it brings, individually and at societal level (Urban, 2015; Moss,2014; Campbell-Barr and Nygard, 2014) There is a certain safety and comfortableness to Sameness (Deleuze and Guattari, 1987; Olsson, 2009) If children are ‘learning’, we are fulfilling our role to imbue them with the knowledge needed as fully functioning adults and ‘good citizens’ Sameness appeals to our sense of order, need for control and, in the busy, stressful life of the teacher, is easy to plan for and fulfils Ofsted requirements However, this one-size-fits-all approach to knowledge and learning does not sit comfortably with our understanding of how children learn and develop, nor does it acknowledge and value the voice of children within day-to-day practice What is promoted in this vision of learning is implicit understanding that there are right/wrong ways of learning and knowing, a binary judgement of good/bad, serving to govern and regulate children’s thinking, actions and agency (Deleuze 1994) In this reality, as attested by Cliffe and Solvason (2016) and international studies such as Kuby (2012) and Hargraves (2014), children’s random, unpredictable connections and ideas are often considered ‘off-task’ or ‘wrong’ The ‘Otherness’ of the child’s voice is re-directed to more acceptable pathways, or made to function as ‘Same’; either way we silence that voice rather than listen and honour it In doing so we cheat both the child and ourselves of valuable learning experiences We must ask how we can allow children more freedom to present their ideas and ensure we genuinely hear and value them, without allowing ourselves to become stuck within fixed and binary positions The Deleuzio-Guattarian (1987) notion of rhizonalysis offers an alternative that resists binary right/wrong judgements (Cliffe and Solvason, 2016) Founded on the metaphorical structure of the rhizome, rhizoanalysis affords fresh insights into how we understand and accommodate the complexity and Otherness of children’s thinking and ideas Within this process all learning connections are considered valid and function simultaneously, allowing for multiple pathways to meaning to become possible Through rhizoanalysis children’s experiences are viewed like an interconnected web, with sense and meaning emerging in relationships between each experience - a theory supported by research into neuroconstructivist approaches (Sirois et al., 2008, Westermann et al., 2010) Instead of attempting to curtail the uniqueness, depth and diversity of children’s connections into what we expect and what makes sense to us through adult rationality, rhizoanalysis challenges us to ask instead ‘How does this work?’ and ‘What else might be happening here?’ ‘What new thought does this make it possible to think?’ Resisting the temptation to become fixated on what something is we open the door to what it might make possible This not only provides children with a voice but it supports the notion of children as true partners in their learning and that purposeful knowledge and understanding is a process of negotiated sense and meaning Sellers (2009, p.149) stated that adults “…cannot understand the world as children understand it…we need children to explain their perspectives to us” whilst Grieshebar and McArdle (2010) suggest children never really need adults in order to learn In practice, rather than the balance of power shifting from powerful teacher/educator to powerful child within policy, it still fluctuates uneasily somewhere between the two However, there is much to be said for Freire's (1999) observation that until educators and children meet together as both learner and teacher neither will realise their full potential No, children are not competent researchers, but they are the experts in their own lives Creating a culture of exploration For children to confidently share their ideas we need to recognise issues of power and policy within classrooms Asking children our own questions does not provide opportunity for them to discuss issues that are most pertinent to them So change must happen at the heart of the setting culture, not simply in our choice of research method Waller and Bitou (2011: 12) suggest “There may be an assumption that the tools themselves somehow automatically enable participation The key message from literature is that it is research design and relationships that confer real participation and engagement”; so not a particular data collection method but negotiation of power, respect and the development of shared expectations Ideas of how to collect data are shaped by our views of children's competence, emerging from our values about childhood (McDowall Clark, 2016) Before we can hear what they have to say, children must believe they can formulate their own questions and make their own decisions; Reggio Emilia philosophy provides sound values on which to build such an approach The difference between this and other national and international approaches, is that practitioners no longer take the role of benefactor and dispenser of knowledge Instead, children and adults are seen as fellow researchers, discovering new knowledge side-by-side as teachers ask ‘how can we find this out?’ Rinaldi, a Reggio Emilia spokeswoman, stresses the importance of research as a day-to-day tool within educational environments rather than a field reserved for academics She stresses that it is time for research to “come out of the scientific laboratories, thus ceasing to be a privilege of the few to become the stance, the attitude with which teachers approach the sense and meaning of life” (Rinaldi 2005: 148) Teachers don’t hold ‘answers’ about life any more than nurses, dentists or librarians Their role is not to give answers, but to provide children with skills to find out for themselves How we involve all children? As a research supervisor it is common to see trainee practitioners' research that consults all adults within the setting, then throws in a smattering of children’s views, “more for decoration than illumination” (Roberts, 2017: 143) Often the reasoning is that children cannot really understand the topic, despite the fact that children usually are at the receiving end of the process under scrutiny No, they will not understand the range of approaches to phonics or which are most effective, but they will know which sound activities they enjoy and which they not Children may not understand different approaches to observation and assessment, but wouldn’t it be interesting to compare children’s and teachers' perceptions of what is actually learnt during an activity? Articles 12 & 13 of the United Nations Rights of the Child (UNICEF, undated) state that children’s views should be respected and we should provide them with ways to express those views; authors such as Clark, Kjorholt and Moss (2005) provide useful tools to that But Dockett et al (2009) stress that selecting a representative sample is not enough They say “Children have diverse perspectives, experiences and understandings Choosing to involve some in research and not others can mean this diversity is neither recognised nor respected” (Dockett et al, 2009: 289) So perhaps it is time to rethink the concept of the representative sample Hart (1995) identifies eight levels of research when working with children Most research starts somewhere around tokenism and stops at consultation, where we elicit the views of the child Rarely are decisions about research made with children, let alone initiated by children Wonderful as the Mosaic Approach is (Clark, Kjorholt and Moss, 2005) it is designed as a tool for adults to elicit what they want to find out, about topics they have identified It relies upon adults making the ultimate interpretation Yet examples from published research have found that children have very different perceptions of what, exactly, their data (artwork for example) means, compared to adult interpretations (Punch 2002; Merewether and Fleet, 2014; Palaiologou, 2013) But how is it possible to achieve anything with research without a clearly prescribed direction to take? Be prepared for mess It is claimed that Albert Einstein said “If we knew what it was we were doing it would not be called research, would it?” I hope it is true, but am not wholly certain of the rigour of BrainyQuotes.com It is something I like to use with students to assure them there is no designated path for research - we follow where it leads; to reassure them that in a culture of educational binaries of right and wrong, of memorising correct answers, sometimes it is okay not to know McNiff (2016: 16) compares all research to stepping off a cliff, you have no idea where you will land And if children are involved then the risk of a detour increases tenfold But is that such a bad thing? Gallacher and Gallagher (2008) found some of their most fascinating insights came when children sabotaged their planned data collection techniques So we return to ideas of power and control If we allow children to take an active role in a research project it is unlikely that initial goals will be met (Dona, 2006) No matter how ‘meticulously planned and carefully applied’ (Gallacher and Gallagher, 2008: 503) research approaches are, all best intentions are likely to be turned inside out if children take the reins The more we try to rationalise a certain approach toward researching with children, the more absurd it seems Children are unpredictable, impulsive, unrestrained, everything that bucks against carefully prescribed steps to successful research There is no clear start and end point, no clear path Gollop (2000) reminds me that we not know what children know and not Often they don’t even know So we need to be more ‘early years’ in our approach to research with children, and learn, difficult as it may be, to ‘go with the flow.’ If we are endeavouring to understand children then we should be a little bit more like them embrace messiness and playfulness, be genuinely naïve We should empower children and nurture their research skills by being alongside them as they discover, exploring with them Gallacher and Gallagher (2008: 510-511) argue that we should embrace the childlike position of researching and enjoy its multitudinous possibilities They say: “In contrast to the dominant image of the academic as expert… For us, research is fundamentally a process of muddling through, sometimes feeling lost and out of place, asking stupid questions, being corrected and having our preconceptions destroyed In this way, we cannot deny our incompetence and vulnerabilities: our immaturity And we not want to.” Research with children is messy and problematic, but also it has no limitations Let us hope more of us are brave enough to explore it References Bateman, A (2017) Hearing children's voices through a conversation analysis approach International Journal of Early Years Education, Volume 25, Issue 3, pp 241-253 British Educational Research Association (2018) Ethical guidelines for educational research (4th ed.) Available from: https://www.bera.ac.uk/wp-content/uploads/2018/06/BERA-Ethical-Guidelines-forEducational-Research_4thEdn_2018.pdf?noredirect=1 Campbell Barr, V & Nygard, M (2014) 'Losing sight of the child? Human capital theory and its role for early childhood education and care policies in Finland and England since the mid-1990s' Contemporary Issues in Early childhood (4) 346-359 Clark, A., Kjorholt, T, & Moss, P (Eds.)(2005), Beyond listening: children’s perspectives on early childhood services Bristol: Policy Press Cliffe, J & Solvason, C (2016) Using Rhizomatic Thinking in Early Childhood Pedagogy to Avoid Making Other into Same Available from: http://tactyc.org.uk/reflections/ Dahlberg, G & Moss, P (2005) Ethics and Politics in Early Childhood Education, London:Routledge Falmer Deleuze, G (1994) Difference and repetition, London: Bloomsbury Academic Deleuze, G & Guattari, F (1987) reprinted (2013) A Thousand Plateaus London: Bloomsbury Academic Dockett, S., Einarsdottir, J & Perry, B (2009) Researching with Children: Ethical Tensions Journal of Early Childhood Research, (3), 283-298 Dona, G (2006) Children as research advisor contributions to a "methodology of participation" in researching children in difficult circumstances International Journal of Migration, Health and Social Care, (2) 22-34 Flückiger, B., Dunn, J and Stinson, M (2018) What supports and limits learning in the early years? Listening to the voices of 200 children Australian Journal of Education Vol 62, Issue 2, pp 94 - 107 Freire, P (1999) The Pedagogy of the Heart New York: Continuum Press Gallacher, L & Gallagher, M (2008) Methodological Immaturity in Childhood Research Childhood, 15 (4), 499-516 Gallagher, M (2008) Power is not an evil: rethinking power in participatory methods Children's Geographies, (2), 137-150 Gollop, M M (2000) Interviewing children: a research perspective In: A B Smith, N J Taylor & M M Gollop (Eds.), Children’s voices: research, policy and practice (pp 18-37) New Zealand: Pearson Education Grieshebar, S & McArdle, F (2010) The Trouble with Play, Maidenhead, England: Open University Press Hargraves, V (2014) Complex possibilities: working theories as an outcome for the early childhood curriculum Contemporary issues in Early Childhood, 15(4), 319-328 Hart, R (1995) Children's Participation: From tokenism to citizenship, Florence: UNICEF Innocenti Research Centre James A (2004) Understanding childhood from an interdisciplinary perspective: problems and potentials In: PB Pufall & RP Unsworth (Eds.), Rethinking Childhood London: Rutgers University Press Kuby, C.R (2012) ‘Ok this is hard’: doing emotions in social justice dialogue Education, Citizenship and Social Justice, 8(1), 29 – 42 McDowall Clark, R (2016) Childhood in Society (3rd Ed.) London: Sage Publications McNiff, J (2016) You and Your Action Research Project (4th Ed.) Abingdon: Routledge Merewether, J and Fleet, A (2014) Seeking children’s perspectives: a respectful layered research approach Early Childhood Development and Care, 184 (6), 897-914 Moss P (2014) Transformative Change and Real Utopias in Early Childhood Education: a story of democracy, experimentation and potentiality, London: Routledge Mukherji, P and Albon, D (2010) Research Methods in Early Childhood Sage: London Olsson, L M (2009) Movement and experimentation in young children’s learning, Oxon: Routledge Palaiologou, I (2013) ‘Do we hear what children want to say?’ Ethical praxis when choosing research tools with children under five Early Childhood Development and Care, accessed http://dx.doi.org./10.1080/03004430.2013.809341 accessed 01/07/13 Punch, S (2002) Research with children: the same or different from research with adults? Childhood, (3), 321-341 Rinaldi, C (2005) In dialogue with Reggio Emilia London: RoutledgeFalmer Roberts (2017) Listening to Children and Hearing them In P Christensen, & A James (Eds.), Research with Children Perspectives and practices (3rd ed.) (pp 142-159) Abingdon: Routledge Sellers, M (2009) Re(con)ceiving children in curriculum: Mapping (a) milieu(s) of becoming PhD thesis University of Queensland Available from: https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/09518398.2010.500629? scroll=top&needAccess=true Sirois, S., Spratling, M., Thomas, M., Westermann, G., Mareshcal, D., & Johnson, M (2008) Précis of neuroconstructivism: how the brain constructs cognition, Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31(3), pp 321-31 Solvason, C (2018) ‘Just how important is ethics within early childhood research and professionalism.’ Journal of Early Childhood Education Research, (2), pp 166-176, http://jecer.org/issues/jecer-62-2017-special-issue/ Solvason, C (2017) Learning to be an Ethical Practitioner, in Musgrave, J., Savin-Baden, M & Stobbs, N (eds) Studying for Your Early Years Degree, Critical Publishing: St Albans Solvason, C (2016) ‘Ethicality, research and Emotional Impoverishment in a Technological Era’ in J.McNiff (Ed) Values and Virtues in Higher Education Research, London: Routledge Solvason, C., Cliffe, J & Snowden, M (2017) ‘Researching in School-Creating a Meaningful School/ University Alliance: A Reflection.’ Educational Action Research, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09650792.2017.1388828 UNICEF (Original version 1989, this summary undated) A summary of the United Nations Convention on the Rights of the Child Retrieved from: https://downloads.unicef.org.uk/wpcontent/uploads/2010/05/UNCRC_summary-1.pdf?_ga=2.67312977.1555243689.153606021753205099.1536060217 Urban, M (2015) 'From ‘closing the gap’ to an ethics of affirmation: reconceptualising the role of early childhood services in times of uncertainty' European Journal of Education, 50 (3), pp293-306 Waller, T & Bitou, A (2011) Research with children: three challenges for participatory research in early childhood, European Early Childhood Education Research Journal, 19 (1), pp 5-20 Westerman, G., Thomas, M., Karmiloff-Smith, A (2010) Neuroconstructivism, in Goswami, U (Ed) The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Cognitive Development, 2nd Edition Chichester: WileyBlackwell

Ngày đăng: 20/10/2022, 13:54

w