1. Trang chủ
  2. » Giáo Dục - Đào Tạo

PERSONALITY PROCESSES AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES decreased latent inhibition is associated with increased creative achievement in high functioning individuals

8 4 0

Đang tải... (xem toàn văn)

THÔNG TIN TÀI LIỆU

Thông tin cơ bản

Định dạng
Số trang 8
Dung lượng 364,25 KB

Nội dung

PERSONALITY PROCESSES AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES Decreased Latent Inhibition Is Associated With Increased Creative Achievement in High-Functioning Individuals Shelley H Carson Jordan B Peterson Harvard University University of Toronto Daniel M Higgins Harvard University Reductions in latent inhibition (LI), the capacity to screen from conscious awareness stimuli previously experienced as irrelevant, have been generally associated with the tendency towards psychosis However, “failure” to screen out previously irrelevant stimuli might also hypothetically contribute to original thinking, particularly in combination with high IQ Meta-analysis of two studies, conducted on youthful high-IQ samples, demonstrated that high lifetime creative achievers had significantly lower LI scores than low creative achievers (reffect size ϭ 31, p ϭ 0003, one-tailed) Eminent creative achievers (participants under 21 years who reported unusually high scores in a single domain of creative achievement) were times more likely to have low rather than high LI scores, ␹2(1, N ϭ 25) ϭ 10.69, ␾ ϭ 47, p ϭ 003 1988a, 1988b; Lubow, Ingberg-Sachs, Zalstein-Orda, & Gewirtz, 1992) Recent evidence has suggested, however, that reductions in LI are also associated with the personality trait Openness to Experience (Peterson & Carson, 2000; Peterson, Smith, & Carson, 2002) Openness, in turn, has been consistently associated with divergent thinking and trait creativity (McCrae, 1987) and with creative achievement (King, Walker, & Broyles, 1996) It therefore appears possible that reductions in LI may be associated with increases in human creativity, as suggested by Eysenck (1995) Many researchers (e.g., Simonton, 1988, 1999) have proposed that the cognitive processes of individuals capable of creating the highest achievements in their fields are both qualitatively and quantitatively different from those of normal thinkers (although some, like Weisberg, 1993, dispute the “qualitative” distinction) If qualitative differences exist, however, one potential source of difference in the cognitive processes between eminent creative achievers and other intelligent thinkers may be in the relative attenuation of LI Such attenuation could well increase the number of available mental elements, described by Simonton (1988) as key, in part, to the process of creative discovery Other avenues of research have buttressed this hypothesis Dykes and McGhie (1976) demonstrated, for example, that creative subjects and schizophrenic subjects were better than controls at identifying items presented on the irrelevant channel of a dichotic shadowing task This finding appears to support Dellas and Gaier’s (1970) observation that creative individuals tend not to screen out so-called “irrelevant details.” Furthermore, in a study conducted by Martindale, Anderson, Moore, and West (1996), a How you know but ev’ry Bird that cuts the airy way, Is an immense world of delight, clos’d by your senses five? —William Blake, “A Memorable Fancy,” The Prophetic Books Creative individuals appear characterized in part by the ability to perceive and describe what remains hidden from the view of others Individual variation in latent inhibition (LI), a cognitive inhibitory mechanism discovered by animal experimentalists in the late 1950s, may account for the apparent revelation to the creative mind of what appears “clos’d by the senses five” to others LI refers to the varying capacity of the brain to screen from current attentional focus stimuli previously experienced as irrelevant (Lubow, 1989) The LI phenomenon appears robust across a variety of mammalian species, and its biological underpinnings have been extensively studied (Lubow & Gewirtz, 1995) In humans, reduced LI has generally been associated with susceptibility to or actual acute-phase schizophrenia (Baruch, Hemsley, & Gray, Shelley H Carson and Daniel M Higgins, Department of Psychology, Harvard University; Jordan B Peterson, Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada We thank Melanie Glickson for her research assistance in this study We also thank Robert Sternberg for his helpful comments This research was supported by grants from the Harvard University Department of Psychology and the University of Toronto Department of Psychology Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Shelley H Carson, Department of Psychology, Harvard University, 33 Kirkland Street, Cambridge, Massachusetts 02138 E-mail: carson@wjh.harvard.edu Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2003, Vol 85, No 3, 499 –506 Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc 0022-3514/03/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.3.499 499 500 CARSON, PETERSON, AND HIGGINS small group of high-creative subjects showed reduced galvanic skin response habituation rates to auditory stimuli compared with low-creative subjects This finding, which has been replicated in psychosis-prone populations (Raine, Benishay, Lencz, & Scarpa, 1997), may indicate that highly creative people not precategorize stimuli as irrelevant in the same manner as less creative individuals Finally, Eysenck (1995) has noted that originality (the ability to produce statistically unusual ideas) is conceptually similar to the looseness of associations symptomatic of psychosis Such looseness is presumably a byproduct of the failure of an inhibitory filtering mechanism, functioning to limit associations to those relevant to current task performance Reduced LI scores are theoretically associated with relaxation of this inhibitory mechanism (Gray, Feldon, Rawlins, Hemsley, & Smith, 1991) If reduced LI represents a predisposing factor common to psychosis and to creativity, what then distinguishes the psychotic from the poet? Studies that have associated decreased LI with schizophrenia or schizotypy have typically failed to report subject IQ However, among individuals with a predisposition to psychosis, low IQ generally functions as an unfavorable moderating variable (David, Malmberg, Brandt, Allebeck, & Lewis, 1997; Jones & Offord, 1975) Furthermore, several investigators (e.g., Claridge, 1997; Eysenck, 1995; Berenbaum & Fujita, 1994) have suggested that superior intellectual qualities, such as high IQ, may moderate the expression of a predisposition to psychosis in the highly creative individual It seems reasonable to propose that some psychological phenomena might be pathogenic in the presence of decreased intelligence, psychometrically defined, but normative or even abnormally useful in the presence of increased intelligence The purpose of the present studies was therefore to (a) determine if a variety of indicators of individual creativity, particularly creative achievement, would be associated with reduced LI; (b) determine if reduced LI in tandem with high IQ would predict higher general creative achievement scores than either factor in isolation; and (c) determine if a combination of high IQ and reduced LI would identify those participants who have made especially noteworthy contributions to their respective creative fields We limited our two participant samples to a creatively diverse but high-IQ population to substantively increase the probability of assessing individuals who were genuinely highly creative, because a wide body of research (summarized in Eysenck, 1995) has already identified IQ as a critically important factor in creative achievement This body of research has suggested that there may be an IQ threshold (usually described as an IQ of approximately 120 points) associated with true creative ability (for a review, see Sternberg & O’Hara, 1999) We also present a meta-analysis of the combined results of both of our studies to provide a more accurate estimate of effect size for LI and, as well, we describe the methods and results of an additional analysis of eminent creative achievers versus highly intelligent noncreative individuals from the pooled sample STUDY 1: LI, CREATIVE ACHIEVEMENT, AND OTHER MEASURES OF CREATIVITY to achievement, mental processes (Torrance, 1968), a personality trait or combination of traits (Gough, 1979; McCrae, 1987), and consensually approved novel products (Amabile, 1996) We are particularly interested in lifetime creative achievement, per se, because such achievement is beneficial not only to the individual but to society as well Furthermore, other measures of creativity, including creative personality traits and creative mental process measures (divergent thinking), have been found to be elevated in certain nonproductive disordered populations as well (e.g., Cramond, 1994; Keefe & Magaro, 1980) and thus may not be unique to highly creative individuals However, our initial examination of the LI– creativity association used multiple measures of creativity designed to cover a wide range of concepts, including a lifetime creative achievement measure (the Creative Achievement Questionnaire [CAQ]; Carson, Peterson & Higgins, 2003), a mental process measure (divergent thinking tasks; Torrance, 1968), and a personality measure (the Creative Personality Scale [CPS]; Gough, 1979).1 For Study 1, we hypothesized that (a) high scorers relative to low scorers on the CAQ would be characterized by attenuated LI We also hypothesized that (b) high scorers on the CPS and the divergent thinking tasks would demonstrate attenuated LI relative to low scorers Finally, we hypothesized that (c) low LI scores combined with high IQ scores would predict high CAQ scores Method Participants Eighty-six Harvard undergraduates (33 men, 53 women), with a mean age of 20.7 years (SD ϭ 3.3) participated in the study All were recruited from sign-up sheets posted on campus Participants were paid an hourly rate Procedure Participants were assigned randomly to either the preexposed (n ϭ 57) or the nonpreexposed (n ϭ 29; a ratio of approximately 2:1) LI condition During a pretest interview in which demographic information was collected, participants were assessed for outward signs of depression, anxiety, or boredom They were also questioned about alcohol and/or caffeine intake prior to the lab testing Two participants were rescheduled because of recent caffeine intake All participants then completed creativity measures, IQ tests, personality tests, and the LI task Creativity Testing Creativity testing consisted of three phases: the CAQ, the divergent thinking tasks, and the CPS CAQ The CAQ is an empirically sound measure of lifetime creative accomplishment in the fields of art and science The instrument has demonstrated test–retest reliability in the range of r ϭ 85 as well as good convergent, discriminant, and predictive validity Participants check off actual achievements in 10 separate domains of creative accomplishment (e.g., “My work has won a prize or prizes at a juried art show”) Scores for individual accomplishments within a domain are weighted according to the One of the major obstacles to creativity research has been the variety of diverse concepts and definitions associated with the term creative (Brown, 1989), a descriptor that applies with equal facility An additional measure related to creativity, the personality variable Openness to Experience, was also assessed for its relationship to LI in this sample However, analysis of this relationship has been reported elsewhere (Peterson & Carson, 2000) LATENT INHIBITION AND CREATIVE ACHIEVEMENT level of achievement attained, as ranked by experts within that domain Weighted scores for each domain are summed to provide a total creative achievement score (Carson et al., 2003) Divergent thinking tasks Four validated divergent thinking tasks were adapted from Torrance (1968) Participants were given to write down their responses to each task, such as naming alternate uses for a common object Three aspects of divergent thinking were assessed: fluency (number of responses generated), originality (unusualness of responses, based on the statistical infrequency of each individual response within the current sample), and flexibility (number of different categories of response and the number of category changes) Fluency, originality, and flexibility scores were z scored and summed to produce a divergent thinking score for each subject CPS The CPS (Gough, 1979) consists of a set of 30 items from the Adjective Check List (Gough & Heilbrun, 1965), which has predicted high levels of creativity across multiple studies and diverse samples Participants describe themselves by checking off 18 positively and 12 negatively scored adjectives IQ Testing Participants completed the Vocabulary and Block Design subtests of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Revised (WAIS-R; Wechsler, 1981) Raw scores were age scaled, combined to form a composite, and converted to a full-scale equivalent, using standard guidelines (Brooker & Cyr, 1986) IQ estimates compiled from this “short form” correlate at r ϭ 91 with full-scale WAIS-R IQ scores (Brooker & Cyr, 1986) Because IQ scores using the short form typically overestimate IQ by points (Brooker & Cyr, 1986), IQ scores were adjusted by subtracting points from the total for each participant LI Task Participants were seated in a quiet, semidarkened room and told that they were to participate in two auditory discrimination tasks They then donned stereo earphones and adjusted the volume to ensure clarity and comprehensibility Participants in the preexposed condition were then shown a two-part video version of a well-validated and commonly used auditory LI task, after Lubow et al (1992) In Part 1, the preexposure phase, a list of 30 nonsense syllables (the masking material) was presented times at a normal rate of speech with no indication of the termination and start of each repetition White noise bursts (the target stimuli) from to s in duration were superimposed randomly 31 times over the course of the recording, at approximately twothirds the volume of the nonsense syllables Participants listened to the recording through earphones and were asked to determine how many times they heard a selected nonsense syllable In Part (the test phase), the recording of the nonsense syllables and the white noise bursts was replayed in identical form while yellow disks arranged in rows on a black scoreboard were revealed individually on the video screen Each yellow disk appeared prior to the offset of the target stimulus Participants were asked to determine what auditory stimulus signaled the appearance of the yellow disks The individual’s score for the task (trials to rule identification) was determined by the number of yellow disks visible on the screen (maximum 31) when the correct answer was given Participants in the nonpreexposed condition were shown an identical videotape, except that the target stimulus was absent from the preexposure phase of the task Traditionally, LI is demonstrated (and the LI test validated) within a population when participants of that population tested in the preexposed condition take more trials to learn the association of the target stimulus with the yellow disks than similar participants tested in the nonpreexposed condition (Lubow et al., 1992) Thus, lower LI scores in the preexposed 501 condition suggest attenuated LI, whereas higher scores suggest intact or “normal” LI Results Table lists the zero-order correlations of all creativity measures with each other as well as with IQ Creative Achievement and LI Participants were divided into low- (n ϭ 45, M ϭ 6.7, SD ϭ 3.9) and high- (n ϭ 41, M ϭ 27.0, SD ϭ 11.3) creative achievement groups by CAQ score median split.2 Members of the low-creative achievement group in the preexposed condition (n ϭ 28) scored significantly higher on the LI task than the low-creative achievers in the nonpreexposed condition (n ϭ 17), indicating a significant LI effect, t(43) ϭ Ϫ2.03, p ϭ 04, d ϭ 62 (see Figure 1) Members of the high-creative achievement group in the preexposed condition (n ϭ 29), by contrast, did not score significantly higher on the LI task than their counterparts in the nonpreexposed condition (n ϭ 12), indicating an attenuation of LI in the high-creative achievement group, t(39) ϭ Ϫ.020, p ϭ 96, d ϭ 01 An analysis of variance (ANOVA) indicated significant differences in LI scores due to the effects of high- and low-creative achievement, F(1, 82) ϭ 4.09, p ϭ 05, ␩2 ϭ 05 The main effect for condition, F(1, 82) ϭ 2.12, p ϭ 15, ␩2 ϭ 03, and the interaction effect between CAQ and condition, F(1, 82) ϭ 2.04, p ϭ 16, ␩2 ϭ 02, were not significant Because LI scores were clearly bimodal rather than normally distributed (see Figure 2), nonparametric analyses (Mann–Whitney U) were also conducted and yielded similar results: Low-creative achievement participants demonstrated intact LI (Z ϭ Ϫ1.86, p ϭ 06), whereas high-creative achievement participants demonstrated reduced LI (Z ϭ Ϫ.043, p ϭ 96) As hypothesized, the mean LI score of the low-creative achievement group (n ϭ 28, M ϭ 21.8, SD ϭ 10.4) was significantly higher than that of the high-creative achievement group (n ϭ 29, M ϭ 14.3, SD ϭ 8.8) in the preexposed condition, t(55) ϭ 2.93, p ϭ 006, d ϭ 79 (nonparametric Mann–Whitney U: Z ϭ Ϫ2.54, p ϭ 01) (see Figure 3) LI and Measures of Divergent Thinking and Creative Personality A comparison of means indicated that LI scores in the preexposed condition were significantly reduced in the high-scoring groups (determined by median split) of the CPS and the originality dimension of the divergent thinking tasks as well as in overall divergent thinking (see Table 2) Nonparametric Mann–Whitney U tests revealed similar results LI and IQ as Factors of Creative Achievement The mean IQ of the sample was 128.1 points (SD ϭ 10.3), with a range of 97 to 148 points To determine whether LI and IQ would “Median split” is traditionally used in LI research (e.g Baruch et al., 1988a, 1988b; Lubow et al., 1992) and thus is included here However, median splits arguably lack theoretical meaning A theoretically driven division of high- and low-creative achievers is presented in the ANALYSIS OF EMINENT CREATIVE ACHIEVERS section CARSON, PETERSON, AND HIGGINS 502 Table Correlations of Creativity and IQ Measures Measure CAQ CPS Diverg Fluency Orig Flex CPS Diverg Fluency Orig Flex IQ 33* 47* 38* 46* 37* 13 29* 11 32* 34* 26* 85* 86* 87* 10 59* 62* Ϫ.03 63* 19 08 Note CAQ ϭ Creative Achievement Questionnaire; CPS ϭ Creative Personality Scale; Diverg ϭ divergent thinking; Orig ϭ originality; Flex ϭ flexibility * p Ͻ 05 jointly predict creative achievement scores, we regressed LI scores (preexposed condition) and IQ scores on the creative achievement scores of the highest and lowest quartiles of CAQ scorers (n ϭ 30) Negative LI scores and positive IQ scores jointly predicted 26% of the variance in creative achievement scores, F(2, 27) ϭ 4.89, p ϭ 006, R2 ϭ 26, with LI scores alone accounting for 18% of the total CAQ variance, F(1, 28) ϭ 6.09, p ϭ 02, R2 ϭ 18.3 An additional analysis of the CAQ, IQ, and LI relationship is offered below in Study and, cumulatively, in the section ANALYSIS OF EMINENT CREATIVE ACHIEVERS STUDY 2: LI AND CREATIVE ACHIEVEMENT In Study 2, we attempted to replicate our findings within the Harvard population, concentrating primarily on the relationship of LI to lifetime creative achievement Because we had already established the existence of an LI effect for this population using a nonpreexposed control group (and because the LI scores of high creative achievers did not differ significantly from low achievers in the control group), we decided to concentrate on individual difference scores within the preexposed condition We also wished to control for the chance that participants would respond to the self-report measure of creative achievement, the CAQ, in a self-enhancing manner We therefore administered the Figure Bimodal distribution of latent inhibition scores in the preexposed condition in Studies and Marlowe–Crowne Social Desirability Scale (MCSD; Crowne & Marlowe, 1960), a measure of the tendency of responders to tailor their responses to appear socially acceptable Method Participants Ninety-six Harvard undergraduates (53 men, 43 women), with a mean age of 20.1 years (SD ϭ 1.6) participated in the study All were registered in a psychology course and received course credit for participation Figure Creative Achievement Questionnaire (CAQ) and latent inhibition (LI) scores in the preexposed and nonpreexposed conditions Error bars represent the standard error (positive value only) for the mean LI score of each high–low CAQ ϫ Condition group The regression of IQ and LI (preexposed) on the entire sample was as follows: F(2, 58) ϭ 2.84, p ϭ 07, R2 ϭ 10, with LI (preexposed) accounting for 9% of the variance LATENT INHIBITION AND CREATIVE ACHIEVEMENT 503 accounting for 13% of the total CAQ variance, F(1, 43) ϭ 6.54, p ϭ 01, R2 ϭ 13.4 META-ANALYSIS: LI AND CREATIVE ACHIEVEMENT The results of analyses of the relationship between creative achievement scores and LI scores from Studies and were combined using meta-analytic techniques (Rosenthal & Rosnow, 1991) Because both studies used samples of similar IQ and creative achievement, Studies and were weighted equally in the meta-analysis When LI scores were compared between high-CAQ and low-CAQ groups (median split), the combined results of the two studies yielded highly significant differences with an effect size of r ϭ 31 ( p ϭ 0003, one-tailed; see Table 3) ANALYSIS OF EMINENT CREATIVE ACHIEVERS Figure Latent inhibition (LI) scores (preexposed condition) of highand low-creative achievers as measured by the Creative Achievement Questionnaire (CAQ) Error bars represent the standard error (positive value only) for the mean LI score (preexposed) of each high–low CAQ group Procedure All participants completed a computerized version of the CAQ identical to the paper-and-pencil version used in Study as well as a computerized version of the MCSD In subsequent lab sessions, participants were administered the LI task in the experimental condition and the IQ tests Both the LI and the IQ procedures were identical to those used in Study Results Four participants were eliminated from the study because of MCSD scores more than standard deviations above the mean, indicating a strong possibility of self-enhancement in responses on the CAQ Creative Achievement and LI Participants were divided into low-creative achievement (low CAQ; n ϭ 47, M ϭ 4.9, SD ϭ 3.1) and high-creative achievement (high CAQ; n ϭ 45, M ϭ 22.3, SD ϭ 9.7) groups by median split, as in Study (see Figure 3) As predicted, participants in the high-CAQ group had significantly lower LI scores (M ϭ 12.8, SD ϭ 10.1) than did participants in the low-CAQ group (M ϭ 17.6, SD ϭ 11.4); t(90) ϭ 2.13, p ϭ 04, d ϭ 45 (Mann– Whitney U: Z ϭ 2.09, p ϭ 04) LI and IQ as Factors of Creative Achievement Eighty-two of the 96 participants completed the IQ tasks The mean IQ of the sample was 124.6 points (SD ϭ 11.4), ranging from 100 to 148 points As in Study 1, we regressed LI scores (preexposed condition) and IQ scores on the upper and lower quartiles of CAQ scores (n ϭ 46) Negative LI scores and positive IQ scores jointly predicted 20% of the variance in CAQ scores, F(2, 43) ϭ 5.33, p ϭ 006, R2 ϭ 20, with LI scores alone The results of Studies and indicate that participants who scored higher in overall creative achievement exhibited significantly lower LI scores than participants who did not score high in creative achievement However, a high score in combined areas of creative achievement may not necessarily be indicative of the likelihood of truly eminent achievement in a specified field One of the defining characteristics of the truly eminent creative achiever, however, is the production of a significant accomplishment in his or her respective field at a young age (Ludwig, 1995) By pooling the participants from both studies, we hoped to identify enough eminent creative achievers (those who made a truly substantive contribution to a single domain of creative endeavor before the age of 21) to allow for analysis of LI and IQ We defined a score of at least 12 points on the CAQ in any one of the 10 individual domains of measured creative achievement as constituting a clearly significant contribution to a creative field The minimum levels of achievement that qualified as a significant accomplishment according to this criterion included having a novel or book of poetry published and sold, having a musical composition recorded and sold, having a prototype invention patented and built, having a private showing of original artwork at a recognized gallery, or winning a scholarship or national prize for a scientific discovery We then compared the LI scores of the identified group of eminent creative achievers with the scores of the group of individuals who demonstrated minimal creative achievement Method Participants included pooled subjects from Studies and who met the following criteria: (a) They were members of the preexposed condition of the LI task, and (b) they had scored either 12 or more points on any single domain of the CAQ (eminent creative achievers; n ϭ 25), or had a total CAQ score of or below (controls; n ϭ 23) Eminent creative achievers included artists, composers, writers, inventors, dramatists, scientists, and choreographers Mean LI scores of eminent achievers were then compared with the LI scores of the control subjects The regression of IQ and LI (preexposed) on the entire sample was as follows: F(2, 79) ϭ 2.70, p ϭ 07, R2 ϭ 07, with LI (preexposed) accounting for 5% of the variance CARSON, PETERSON, AND HIGGINS 504 Table Latent Inhibition (LI) Scores of the High Versus Low Groups of Individual Measures of Creativity (Based on Median Split) Measure CPS M SD Diverg M SD Orig M SD Fluency M SD Flex M SD LI score of high group LI score of low group 11.0 7.0 20.8 10.1 15.6 9.5 21.4 10.9 15.1 9.7 21.5 10.3 16.4 9.5 21.1 11.5 16.7 9.5 19.6 11.4 t df p d Z 3.63 47 2.09 p Ͻ 01 1.06 Ϫ3.48 Ͻ 01 52 04 0.58 Ϫ1.78 08 2.34 52 02 0.65 Ϫ2.26 02 1.58 52 12 0.43 Ϫ1.37 18 1.01 52 32 0.28 Ϫ0.88 38 Note Z ϭ Mann–Whitney U test; CPS ϭ Creative Personality Scale; Diverg ϭ divergent thinking; Orig ϭ originality; Flex ϭ flexibility Results LI and Eminent Creative Achievers The LI scores of the eminent creative achievers (M ϭ 11.1, SD ϭ 7.6) were significantly lower than the LI scores of the control group (M ϭ 19.4, SD ϭ 10.5), t(46) ϭ 3.17, p ϭ 004, d ϭ 93 (Mann–Whitney U: Z ϭ 2.65, p ϭ 008) Whereas control subjects were equally likely to display either high or low LI scores (on the basis of the manifest division in the LI distribution), eminent creative achievers were seven times more likely to have low rather than high LI scores, ␹2(1, N ϭ 25) ϭ 10.79, ␾ ϭ 47, p ϭ 001 (see Figure 4) LI and IQ as Factors of Eminent Creative Achievement As mentioned above, a number of researchers have noted a correlation between creative ability and IQ levels below 120, whereas correlations between creativity and IQs above 120 are generally less significant This threshold theory suggests that an IQ of 120 may be necessary but not sufficient for creative achievement (for a review, see Sternberg & O’Hara, 1999; for an alternative view, see Lubinski, Webb, Morlock, & Benbow, 2001) In our sample, the mean IQ for the eminent creative achiever group was above the 120 threshold (M ϭ 128.6, SD ϭ 8.3), whereas the mean IQ for the control group was just below the 120 threshold (M ϭ 118.3, SD ϭ 11.9) Twenty-one of the 25 eminent achievers had IQs above the 120 threshold, whereas 11 of the 23 controls had IQs above the threshold The combined sample of eminent creative achievers and controls was divided into high-IQ (M ϭ 130.0, SD ϭ 6.6, n ϭ 32) and moderate-IQ (M ϭ 111.1, SD ϭ 7.6, n ϭ 16) groups on the basis of the proposed threshold score of IQ 120 The combined sample was also divided into high-LI (M ϭ 27.6, SD ϭ 3.8, n ϭ 16) and low-LI (M ϭ 6.6, SD ϭ 4.7, n ϭ 32) groups on the basis of the naturally occurring split in bimodal LI scores in the preexposed condition A ϫ factorial ANOVA examined the CAQ scores using high LI/low LI and high IQ/moderate IQ as factors The results indicated a significant difference among the creative achievement scores of the groups, F(3, 44) ϭ 8.58, p Ͻ 001, ␩2 ϭ 37, with the low-LI/high-IQ group demonstrating substantially higher CAQ scores than all other groups, as predicted (see Figure Table Difference in Mean Latent Inhibition Scores of High Versus Low Creative Achievers: A Meta-Analysis Study t Effect size p 2.93a 2.13b 400 219 314 0025 018 0003 Total Note All p values are one-tailed a df ϭ 55 b df ϭ 90 Figure Contingency table of high and low latent inhibition (LI) scores (preexposed) for eminent creative achievers versus controls LATENT INHIBITION AND CREATIVE ACHIEVEMENT 5) The main effects for both LI, F(1, 44) ϭ 7.30, p ϭ 01, ␩2 ϭ 14, and IQ, F(1, 44) ϭ 5.44, p ϭ 02, ␩2 ϭ 11, were significant, and the interaction between the two factors approached significance, F(1, 44) ϭ 3.47, p ϭ 07, ␩2 ϭ 07 When we regressed LI (preexposed condition) and IQ scores on the CAQ scores, negative LI scores and positive IQ scores jointly predicted almost one third of the variance in creative achievement scores, F(2, 45) ϭ 9.55, p ϭ 0003, R2 ϭ 30, with LI scores alone accounting for 19% of the total CAQ variance, F(1, 46) ϭ 10.53, p ϭ 002, R2 ϭ 19 The LI ϫ IQ interaction was highly significant, F(1, 46) ϭ 15.81, p ϭ 008, ␩2 ϭ 98 GENERAL DISCUSSION The results of these studies and analyses indicate a substantial and significant relationship between a variety of indicators of creativity and reduced LI In Study 1, significant differences in LI scores emerged between high- and low-creative participants on measures of creative achievement, creative personality, and the originality facet of divergent thinking The results of Study replicated the relationship between creative achievement and reduced LI A meta-analysis of the two studies indicated a nontrivial and highly significant relationship between creative achievement and reduced LI, and an analysis of eminent creative achievers (relative to noncreative control subjects) suggested a nearuniversal reduction in LI in this group How we reconcile the fact that reduced LI scores in humans, which have previously been associated with psychotic states or with psychotic proneness, are associated with high levels of creativity in these studies? The results provide substantial although circumstantial evidence that high IQ might serve as a beneficial moderating factor in the expression of LI as either a deficit in selective attention or a facilitator of creativity In all of our studies and analyses, high IQ, when combined with low LI, was associated with increased creative achievement These results are particularly stunning in the analysis of eminent achievers and high-functioning controls High IQ clearly appeared to augment the tendency toward high creative achievement characteristic of low-LI individuals Figure Creative Achievement Questionnaire (CAQ) scores of high– low latent inhibition (LI) and high–moderate IQ groups in pooled eminent achievers and controls Error bars represent the standard error (positive value only) for the mean CAQ score of each high–low LI ϫ IQ group 505 These results lend support to the theory that there may be qualitative (e.g., failure to filter out irrelevant stimuli) as well as quantitative (e.g., high IQ) differences in the processes underlying creative versus normal cognition These results also support the theory that highly creative individuals and psychotic-prone individuals may possess neurobiological similarities, perhaps genetically determined, that present either as psychotic predisposition on the one hand or as unusual creative potential on the other on the basis of the presence of moderating cognitive factors such as high IQ (e.g., Berenbaum & Fujita, 1994; Dykes & McGhie, 1976; Eysenck, 1995) These moderating factors may allow an individual to override a “deficit” in early selective attentional processing with a high-functioning mechanism at a later, more controlled level of selective processing The highly creative individual may be privileged to access a greater inventory of unfiltered stimuli during early processing, thereby increasing the odds of original recombinant ideation Thus, a deficit that is generally associated with pathology may well impart a creative advantage in the presence of other cognitive strengths such as high IQ One of the limitations of the present research was the use of a single measure of LI We are currently developing a multimethod procedure for assessing LI that will hopefully further clarify the relationship between reduced LI and creativity in future research We are also investigating other possible moderating factors (including working memory capacity and personality dimensions) that may have an impact on the creativity–LI relationship References Amabile, T M (1996) Creativity in context Boulder, CO: Westview Press Baruch, I., Hemsley, D R., & Gray, J A (1988a) Differential performance of acute and chronic schizophrenics in a latent inhibition task Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 176, 598 – 606 Baruch, I., Hemsley, D R., & Gray, J A (1988b) Latent inhibition and “psychotic proneness” in normal subjects Personality and Individual Differences, 9, 777–783 Berenbaum, H., & Fujita, F (1994) Schizophrenia and personality: Exploring the boundaries and connections between vulnerability and outcome Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 148 –158 Brooker, B H., & Cyr, J J (1986) Tables for clinicians to use to convert WAIS-R Short Forms Journal of Clinical Psychology, 42, 983 Brown, R T (1989) Creativity: What are we to measure? In J A Glover, R R Ronning, & C R Reynolds (Eds.), Handbook of creativity (pp 3–32) New York: Plenum Press Carson, S., Peterson, J B., & Higgins, D (2003) Validity, reliability, and factor structure of the Creative Achievement Questionnaire Manuscript submitted for publication Claridge, G (Ed.) (1997) Schizotypy: Implications for illness and health New York: Oxford University Press Cramond, B (1994) Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and creativity: What is the connection? Journal of Creative Behavior, 28, 193–210 Crowne, D P., & Marlowe, D (1960) A new scale of social desirability independent of psychopathology Journal of Consulting Psychology, 24, 349 –354 David, A S., Malmberg, A., Brandt, L., Allebeck, P., & Lewis, G (1997) IQ and risk for schizophrenia: A population-based cohort study Psychological Medicine, 27, 1311–1323 Dellas, M., & Gaier, E L (1970) Identification of creativity: The individual Psychological Bulletin, 73, 55–73 Dykes, M., & McGhie, A (1976) A comparative study of attentional 506 CARSON, PETERSON, AND HIGGINS strategies of schizophrenic and highly creative normal subjects British Journal of Psychiatry, 128, 50 –56 Eysenck, H J (1995) Creativity as a product of intelligence and personality In D Saklofske & M Zeidner (Eds.), International handbook of personality and intelligence: Perspectives on individual differences (pp 231–247) New York: Plenum Press Gough, H G (1979) A creative personality scale for the Adjective Check List Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1398 –1405 Gough, H G., & Heilbrun, A B (1965) The Adjective Check List manual Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press Gray, J A., Feldon, J., Rawlins, J N P., Hemsley, D R., & Smith, A D (1991) The neuropsychology of schizophrenia Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 14, 1– 84 Jones, M B., & Offord, D R (1975) Independent transmission of IQ and schizophrenia British Journal of Psychiatry, 126, 185–190 Keefe, J A., & Magaro, P A (1980) Creativity and schizophrenia: An equivalence of cognitive processing Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 89, 390 –398 King, L., Walker, L., & Broyles, S (1996) Creativity and the five-factor model Journal of Research in Personality, 30, 189 –203 Lubinski, D., Webb, R M., Morelock, M J., & Benbow, C P (2001) Top in 10,000: A 10-year follow-up of the profoundly gifted Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 718 –729 Lubow, R E (1989) Latent inhibition and conditioned attention theory Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press Lubow, R E., & Gewirtz, J C (1995) Latent inhibition in humans: Data, theory, and implications for schizophrenia Psychological Bulletin, 117, 87–103 Lubow, R E., Ingberg-Sachs, Y., Zalstein-Orda, N., & Gewirtz, J C (1992) Latent inhibition in low and high “psychotic-prone” normal subjects Personality and Individual Differences, 15, 563–572 Ludwig, A M (1995) The price of greatness: Resolving the creativity and madness controversy New York: Guilford Press Martindale, C., Anderson, K., Moore, K., & West, A N (1996) Creativity, oversensitivity, and the rate of habituation Personality and Individual Differences, 20, 423– 427 McCrae, R R (1987) Creativity, divergent thinking, and openness to experience Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 1258 – 1263 Peterson, J B., & Carson, S (2000) Latent inhibition and openness to experience in a high-achieving student population Personality and Individual Differences, 28, 323–332 Peterson, J B., Smith, K W., & Carson, S (2002) Openness and Extraversion are associated with reduced latent inhibition: Replication and commentary Personality and Individual Differences, 33, 1137–1147 Raine, A., Benishay, D., Lencz, T., & Scarpa, A (1997) Abnormal orienting in schizotypal personality disorder Schizophrenia Bulletin, 23, 75– 82 Rosenthal, R., & Rosnow, R L (1991) Essentials of behavioral research: Methods and data analysis New York: McGraw-Hill Simonton, D K (1988) Scientific genius: A psychology of science New York: Cambridge University Press Simonton, D K (1999) Origins of genius: Darwinian perspectives on creativity New York: Oxford University Press Sternberg, R J., & O’Hara, L A (1999) Creativity and intelligence In R J Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of creativity (pp 251–272) New York: Cambridge University Press Torrance, E P (1968) Examples and rationales of test tasks for assessing creative abilities Journal of Creative Behavior, 2, 165–178 Wechsler, D (1981) WAIS-R Manual: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale— Revised San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation Weisberg, R W (1993) Creativity: Beyond the myth of genius New York: Freeman Received February 6, 2002 Revision received January 16, 2003 Accepted January 21, 2003 Ⅲ ... when combined with low LI, was associated with increased creative achievement These results are particularly stunning in the analysis of eminent achievers and high- functioning controls High IQ... toward high creative achievement characteristic of low-LI individuals Figure Creative Achievement Questionnaire (CAQ) scores of high? ?? low latent inhibition (LI) and high? ??moderate IQ groups in pooled... Figure Contingency table of high and low latent inhibition (LI) scores (preexposed) for eminent creative achievers versus controls LATENT INHIBITION AND CREATIVE ACHIEVEMENT 5) The main effects

Ngày đăng: 12/10/2022, 15:58

TÀI LIỆU CÙNG NGƯỜI DÙNG

TÀI LIỆU LIÊN QUAN