Tài liệu hạn chế xem trước, để xem đầy đủ mời bạn chọn Tải xuống
1
/ 109 trang
THÔNG TIN TÀI LIỆU
Thông tin cơ bản
Định dạng
Số trang
109
Dung lượng
3,85 MB
Nội dung
Representational Change as a Way to Enhance Mathematics Learning with Understanding Habilitation Thesis Defended at the University of Hamburg, Faculty of Education, Germany Prof Dr Florence Mihaela Singer UPG University of Ploiesti, Romania 2016 Contents Summary Zusammenfassung Introduction 13 Chapter 1: Setting the theme – some prerequisites for representational change 14 1.1 Science development and human development: what they have in common? Paradigm as a key to explain theory change in science Radical shifts in children’s cognitive development 1.2 The Conceptual Change framework 1.3 The Representational Change Framework 1.4 Teaching for representational change: arguments for its sustainability 1.4.1 Children’s innate propensities for processing domain-specific information 1.4.2 Children’s endowment for recursive processes 1.4.3 Representational strategies and bridging 1.4.4 Children’s capacity to shift among representations in problem solving Chapter 2: Theoretical models that support representational change 2.1 The Dynamic Infrastructure of Mind as lever for representational change 2.2 Mental structures and representational change 2.2.1 How does the human mind organize itself? The building of structures 2.2.2 Types of Spontaneous Structures 2.2.3 Aggregate Structures and Knowledge Construction 28 Chapter 3: Frameworks for implementing 41 14 14 15 15 17 21 21 22 25 26 28 33 33 34 39 the representational-change approach 41 3.1 Dynamic Structural Learning 3.2 Extending the sphere: Representational change within a constructivist approach 3.3 Multi-representational teaching as a strategic approach at university level 42 48 52 Chapter Problem posing as part of representational change 55 4.1 A vision on school problem posing in the knowledge society 4.2 Developing a conceptual framework for studying problem solving and posing 4.3 A new framework for studying creativity through problem posing 4.4 A few findings of the problem-posing studies 4.5 Problem-posing: how teachers interact with? 55 56 58 59 62 Chapter Representational change, expertise, and metacognition 5.1 Operations and dynamics 5.2 Viewing the student as an expert-like learner 5.3 Looking ahead into the research of learning effectiveness 63 ANNEX: Resources for representational change – some examples 69 References 63 65 66 97 Acknowledgements 108 APPENDIX: Full Papers 109 Representational Change as a Way to Enhance Mathematics Learning with Understanding Summary The general aim of the present thesis is to draw on possible strategies to improve learning for new generations of students who are exposed to the actual phenomenon of almost exponential increase of information in terms of both amount and accessibility Can domain-specific expertise be enhanced with appropriate training, across ages? How children build new knowledge, and develop mathematical understanding? What meaning does the relationship between concrete and abstract have in a digital world? These are some of the questions addressed by the present theoretical framework The experimental research and the theoretical underpinnings gathered under its umbrella convey to the following hypothesis: if the knowledge advancement today is dynamic, interdependent, mediated by technology, and mostly unpredictable, and the school learning needs to be better connected to everyday-life contexts, then a possible solution for more effective teaching of mathematics might be training students’ cognitive capabilities through processes that emphasize representational change The theoretical approach of representational change refers to how individuals’ existing knowledge is adapted to allow facing new intellectual challenges, and particularly, to how new knowledge is generated in individuals From a pragmatic view, representational change offers contexts for more effective teaching through processes that stimulate flexibility of mind by operating with adaptive representations The present thesis is structured in five chapters, respectively: Chapter 1: Setting the theme – some prerequisites for representational change; Chapter 2: Theoretical models that support representational change; Chapter 3: Frameworks for implementing representational change; Chapter 4: Problem posing as part of representational change; and Chapter 5: Representational change, expertise, and metacognition In addition, an Annex titled Resources for representational change – some examples offers a few sets of multiple representations and strategies for school contexts at various ages, which have been shown as having potential for enhancing mathematics learning Setting the theme – prerequisites for representational change Within this chapter, the focus is on explaining how the concept of representational change appeared and developed The explanations try to shed new light on convergences and divergences that first guided the conceptual change approach in cognitive science and later on in education and learning More specific, some facts in the history of science and some relevant changes in human cognitive development are analyzed in parallel, emphasizing aspects that generated the conceptual change approach Revealing some limits of its application into mathematics teaching, an extension of the conceptual change theory towards a representational change (RC) approach that may compensate those limitations is proposed Then, a closer look brings new studies into the scene, showing the usefulness of a representational-change perspective for effective learning The question to be answered next is if a RC approach is feasible for students of various ages, in other words: if children have the necessary capacities to process a curriculum that is focused on representational change The answer to this question is given by reviewing a gamut of studies that discusses: children’s innate propensities; children’s endowment for recursive processes; children’s spontaneous bridging ability; and their intuitive capacity to shift among representations in problem solving1 Theoretical models that support representational change Drawing on recent research in cognitive psychology and neuroscience, Chapter provides brief descriptions of two theoretical models developed by the author, which suggest possible explanations on how the human mind works for acquiring knowledge and how it builds structures To a large extent, learning mathematics suppose practicing However, given the time constraints, what to practice in order to enhance learning? The first section of Chapter gives a particular answer to this general question This answer refers to the Dynamic Infrastructure of Mind seen as an information processing mechanism responsible for the cognitive representational power The clusters of operations that constitute the Dynamic Infrastructure of Mind (DIM) offer modalities for training representational capacities in students, in domain-specific situations The features of this model are discussed in the context of recent debates in cognitive psychology related to universalities of mind (cross-domaingeneral processing mechanisms versus domain-specific processors), innateness (inborn characteristics versus environmentally driven acquisitions), and modularity (the mind consists of pre-defined encapsulated modules versus progressive modularization), but the accent is put on the model functionality for effective learning The dynamic infrastructure of mind acts as a domain-general mechanism that progressively specializes with development through learning within environmental interactions The second section of this chapter, titled Mental structures and representational change, goes further in explaining the mechanisms that allow, enhance, or hinder the representational power of mind by describing various types of cognitive structures identified in mathematics learning This chapter reviews a few studies that explore how students activate and use various structures while solving or posing problems These structures have been revealed by interviewing and observing students of various ages The conclusions obtained allowed inferring that, while solving challenging problems, students navigate among concurrent representations and select the most adaptive one for the task at hand Descriptions of these representations – organized as cognitive structures – are provided in a few cases Their organization helps understanding advantages and risks of different learning pathways2 Singer, F M (2012) Boosting the Young Learners' Creativity: Representational Change as a Tool to Promote Individual Talents (Plenary lecture) In The 7th International Group for Mathematical Creativity and Giftedness (MCG) International Conference Proceedings Busan, South Korea: MCG, p 3-26 Singer, F M (2010) Children’s Cognitive Constructions: From Random Trials to Structures In Jared A Jaworski (Ed.), Advances in Sociology Research, vol 6, 1-35 Singer, F M (2009) The Dynamic Infrastructure of Mind - a Hypothesis and Some of its Applications, New Ideas in Psychology, 27(1), 48–74, DOI:10.1016/j.newideapsych.2008.04.007 Singer, F M., Voica, C (2008) Between perception and intuition: thinking about infinity, The Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 27, pp 188-205 Frameworks for implementing the representational change approach Previous experiments have shown that teachers can relatively easy apply some tasks that allow multiple representations The question is to what extent the RC framework can be used on a large scale, how generalizable it is Briefly, it is about two dimensions: developing dynamic conceptual structures within the curriculum, and organizing the teaching practice in a way that generates dynamic structures of thinking in students’ minds In practice, a representational-change-based teaching means at least two things: centering the didactical approach on representation as a powerful tool in learning, and bringing abstract concepts to school very early (in an informal way), in order to stimulate the children’s abstracting capacity during development The third chapter of this synthesis is devoted to answer the question: “How to teach based on representational change?” This answer is three-fold First, some strategic approaches for effective teaching are gathered under the name of Dynamic structural learning, which offers a methodology meant to develop representational capacities of each student Further, a framework for organizing the classroom interactions within a constructivist approach is presented Within this framework, the dynamics of interactions in cognitive development are mirrored in a model of the knowledge construction by the learner Third, a multirepresentational training methodology is used to demonstrate how representational-change strategies can duplicate effectively domain-specific learning from the expert teacher to the novice-prospective teacher Snapshots into some experimental programs developed within the present research, which focus RC in primary grades, in secondary school, and at university level, as well as the philosophy beyond these programs are meant to show the power of applying RC in teaching practice in real settings3 Problem Posing as part of representational change As the capacity of changing representations deals with transfer and creativity, a way to train representational capacities is problem posing4 This chapter brings into attention the main aspects revealed by a couple of studies developed by Singer and her team, in which students of various ages have been questioned about their approaches in devising problems It also allows looking at mathematical high-achieving and gifted students, analyzing how they are dealing with representations An important question addressed in this chapter is: What type of creativity should/can be developed in school through mathematics lessons and what might be generalizable to all students? The discussion reveals the specificity of mathematical creativity, uncovers some of Singer, F M., & Moscovici, H (2008) Teaching and learning cycles in a constructivist approach to instruction Teaching and Teacher Education, Vol 24/6, pp 1613-1634, DOI: 10.1016/j.tate.2007.12.002 Singer, M., Sarivan, L (2009) Curriculum Reframed Multiple Inteligences and New Routes to Teaching and Learning in Romanian Universities In J.Q Chen, S Moran, H Gardner (Eds.), Multiple intelligences around the world Pp 230-244 New York: Jossey-Bass, ISBN: 978-0-7879-9760-1 Singer, F M., Ellerton, N., Cai, J (2013) Problem-Posing Research in Mathematics Education: New Questions and Directions Educational Studies in Mathematics 83(1), 1-7 DOI: 10.1007/s10649-013-9478-2 the limits of students’ creative approaches, and raises questions on the appropriateness of some strategies to overcome these limits in social contexts5 Representational change, expertise, and metacognition By learning to structure and restructure within a representational-change approach, children become more able to analyze their own capabilities The so-called “hyper-learning” or “overlearning” phenomenon recorded within the experiments addressing RC approaches in learning seems to confirm the hypothesis that domain-specific expertise can be enhanced with appropriate training, across ages The discussion concerning the relationship between RC and expertise focuses some traits that make meaningful difference between experts and novices in a specific domain, such as: adaptive thinking schemes, complexity of problem-to-solve representation, goal-oriented procedural knowledge, automation that reduces the concentration of attention, and, above all, metacognitive capacities of self-regulation RC is meant to optimize the learning process through building expert-type cognitive behavior and explicitly developing metacognition The last chapter emphasizes these connections and tries to summarize how mathematics as a tool for rational thinking can play an important role in preparing the fluent thinkers needed in a dynamic world This opens up the discussion within contemporary debates related to teaching and learning in the digital era, highlighting that decisions should be taken based on interdisciplinary research More specific, the chapter analyses the situation of the today (and tomorrow) students that are exposed to various information and communication tools as has never happened with previous generations Having this in view, possible consequences of systematically using a RC approach in teaching are discussed The chapter also addresses some limitations of empirical research on this topic and opens the way to new hypotheses that might be validated in relation to the representational-change approach in teaching and learning6 Singer, F.M & Voica, C (2013) A problem-solving conceptual framework and its implications in designing problem-posing tasks Educational Studies in Mathematics DOI: 10.1007/s10649-012-9422-x, 83(1), 9-26 Singer, F.M & Voica, C (2015) Is Problem Posing a Tool for Identifying and Developing Mathematical Creativity? In F.M Singer, N.F Ellerton & J Cai (Eds.) Mathematical Problem Posing: From Research to Effective Practice, NY: Springer, 141-174 Singer, F.M (2007) Beyond Conceptual Change: Using Representations to Integrate Domain-Specific Structural Models in Learning Mathematics Mind, Brain, and Education, 1(2), pp 84-97, DOI: 10.1111/j.1751228X.2007.00009.x Repräsentationsänderung als Möglichkeit das Mathematiklernen mit Verstehen zu verbessern Zusammenfassung Die vorliegende Habilitationsarbeit verfolgt als Hauptziel die Beschreibung und Analyse möglicher Strategien, um die Lernprozesse einer neuen Schüler- und Studentengenerationen, die dem aktuellen Phänomen der fast exponentiellen Informationszunahme, sowohl quantitativ als auch bzgl der Zugänglichkeit ausgesetzt sind, zu verbessern In diesem Zusammenhang stellen sich folgende Fragen: Kann diese domänenspezifische Kompetenz für Jugendliche jedes Alters durch angemessenes Training, verbessert werden? Wie bauen Kinder neues Wissen auf und wie entwickeln sie mathematisches Verständnis? Welche Bedeutung hat die Beziehung zwischen dem Konkreten und dem Abstrakten in einer digitalen Welt? Diese sind einige der Fragen, die im vorliegenden Forschungsrahmen gestellt und untersucht werden Die experimentelle Forschung und die verschiedenen theoretischen Rahmungen, die sie untermauern, führen zu folgender Hypothese: Da heute die Erweiterung des Wissens dynamisch, verflochten, durch Technologie vermittelt, und vor allem unvorhersehbar ist, und da das schulische Lernen besser mit dem Alltagskontext verbunden werden muss, könnte eine mögliche Lösung für einen effektiveren Mathematikunterricht das Training der kognitiven Fähigkeiten der Lernenden durch Prozesse, die Repräsentationsänderungen (representational change) betonen, sein Der theoretische Ansatz der Repräsentationsänderung bezieht sich auf die Art, wie das vorhandene Wissen durch Individuen bei der Bewältigung neuer geistiger Herausforderungen adaptiert wird, insbesondere bzgl der Erzeugung von neuem Wissen durch die Individuen Aus pragmatischer Sicht bietet die Repräsentationsänderung Kontexte für eine effektivere Lehre durch Prozesse, die Flexibilität des Geistes stimulieren, indem sie mit sich anpassenden Repräsentationen arbeiten Die vorliegende Arbeit ist in fünf Kapitel eingeteilt, und zwar wie folgt: Kapitel 1: Vorstellung des Themas - Voraussetzungen für Repräsentationsänderung; Kapitel 2: Theoretische Modelle, die Repräsentationsänderung unterstützen; Kapitel 3: Rahmen für die Umsetzung von Repräsentationsänderung; Kapitel 4: Problemformulierung als Teil der Repräsentationsänderung; Kapitel 5: Repräsentationsänderung, Expertise und Metakognition Darüber hinaus bietet ein Anhang mit dem Titel Ressourcen für Repräsentationsänderung - einige Beispiele einige Zusammenstellungen von mannigfaltigen Repräsentationen und Strategien für schulische Kontexte in verschiedenen Altersstufen, die das Potential für eine Verbesserung des Mathematiklernens haben Vorstellung des Themas - Voraussetzungen für Repräsentationsänderung Das Hauptanliegen dieses Kapitels ist zu darzulegen, wie das Konzept der Repräsentationsänderung (representational change) entstanden ist und entwickelt wurde Die Erklärungen versuchen, neues Licht auf Konvergenzen und Divergenzen zuerst im Ansatz des Konzeptes des begrifflichen Wandels (conceptual change approach) in den Kognitionswissenschaften, und später in den Bereichen Bildung und Lernen, zu werfen Genauer gesagt, werden einige Fakten in der Geschichte der Wissenschaft und einige relevante Veränderungen in der menschlichen kognitiven Entwicklung analysiert, die den Ansatz des begrifflichen Wandels hervorbrachten Einige Einschränkungen der Anwendung dieses Ansatzes im Mathematikunterricht werden dargestellt, und es wird vorgetragen, dass eine Erweiterung der Theorie des begrifflichen Wandels zum Ansatz der Repräsentationsänderung - representational change (RC) approach - diese Einschränkungen kompensieren kann Eine detailliertere Analyse macht die Relevanz der Perspektive der Repräsentationsänderung deutlich Weiter ist die Frage zu beantworten, ob ein RC-Ansatz für Lernende verschiedener Altersstufen möglich ist, mit anderen Worten, ob Kinder über die notwendigen Fähigkeiten zur Arbeit entlang eines Lehrplans verfügen, der auf Repräsentationsänderung aufgebaut ist Die Antwort auf diese Frage wird durch die Überprüfung eine ganzer Reihe von Studien gegeben, die folgende Themen behandeln: angeborene Neigungen der Kinder; die Begabung der Kinder für rekursive Prozesse, ihre spontane Überbrückungsfähigkeit und ihre intuitive Fähigkeit, bei der Problemlösung zwischen verschiedenen Darstellungen zu wechseln7 Theoretische Modelle, die Repräsentationsänderung unterstützen Auf neuere Forschungen in der kognitiven Psychologie und Neurowissenschaften gestützt, präsentiert Kapitel kurze Beschreibungen zweier von der Verfasserin entwickelter theoretischer Modelle, die mögliche Erklärungen bieten, wie der menschliche Geist beim Erwerb von Wissen funktioniert und wie er Strukturen aufbaut Mathematiklernen bedeutet zu einem großen Teil Übung Da zeitliche Einschränkungen jedoch gegeben sind, stellt sich die Frage, was geübt werden soll, um das Lernen zu verbessern Der erste Abschnitt von Kapitel gibt eine spezifische Antwort auf diese allgemeine Frage Diese Antwort bezieht sich auf den Theorierahmen Dynamische Infrastruktur des Geistes (Dynamic Infrastructure of Mind) als Informationsverarbeitungsmechanismus, das als verantwortlich für die kognitive Repräsentationsleistung gesehen wird Die Cluster von Operationen, die die Dynamic Infrastructure of Mind (DIM) darstellen, bieten Modalitäten für das Trainieren der Repräsentationskapazitäten von Lernenden, in domänenspezifischen Kontexten Die Merkmale dieses Modells werden in Zusammenhang mit neueren Diskussionen zur kognitiven Psychologie diskutiert, u.a zu Universalien des Geistes (domänenübergreifende Verarbeitungsmechanismen vs domänenspezifische Prozessoren), zu Angeborenheit (angeborene Eigenschaften im Vergleich zum umweltbedingten Erwerb) und Modularität (der Geist besteht aus vordefinierten eingekapselten Modulen vs progressive Modularisierung), allerdings liegt das Schwergewicht auf der Funktionalität des Modells für die Effektivität des Lernens Die dynamische Infrastruktur des Geistes wirkt als domänenübergreifender Mechanismus, der sich schrittweise während der Entwicklung spezialisiert, durch das Lernen unter verschiedenen Umweltwechselwirkungen Der zweite Teil dieses Kapitels, mit dem Titel Mentale Strukturen und Repräsentationsänderung, vertieft die Erklärung der Mechanismen, die die Repräsentationsleistung des Geistes ermöglichen, verbessern oder verhindern, indem verschiedene Arten von kognitiven Strukturen, die das Mathematiklernen kennzeichnen, beschrieben werden In diesem Kapitel werden einige Studien, die untersuchen, wie die Lernenden verschiedene mentalen Strukturen aktivieren und verwenden, während sie Aufgaben lösen oder stellen, vorgestellt Diese Strukturen wurden durch Befragung und Beobachtung von Lernenden verschiedenen Alters rekonstruiert Die Ergebnisse weisen darauf hin, dass Lernende, während sie anspruchsvolle Aufgaben lösen, sich gleichzeitig zwischen konkurrierenden Darstellungen bewegen und diejenigen wählen, die am besten für Singer, F M (2012) Boosting the Young Learners' Creativity: Representational Change as a Tool to Promote Individual Talents (Plenary lecture) In the 7th MCG Conference Busan, South Korea: MCG, p 3-26 Singer, F.M (2010) Children’s Cognitive Constructions: From Random Trials to Structures In Jared A Jaworski (Ed.), Advances in Sociology Research, vol 6, 1-35 10 Example 15: Using representations to understand irrational numbers The image below shows how to use the compass to draw a segment of length √2 The drawings below help consolidating the understanding of the magnitude of different irrational numbers such as √2 or √8 They also help connecting rational and irrational numbers, as well as algebra and geometry Students are invited to represents many other irrational numbers using grids and knowledge about area, length of sides and diagonals of squares They can this similarly for , using 3D representations Drawings can support the understanding of properties of irrational numbers They can clarify counterintuitive counterexamples, preventing students to develop misconceptions 95 Example 16: Exploring properties of geometrical figures Part of representational change is the understanding of conventions, using them in appropriate contexts, and making transfers within various configurations Task 15.1 Each figure contains some contradictory claims Find them Task 15.2 Find the measures of the angles denoted by letters For each of the figures, students are pushed to move from one configuration to another and depict relevant data In doing so, they are alternating the focus on certain elements of the figure changing the representation through giving different roles to the elements they encounter and organizing them in a logical structured pattern that leads to solving the problem For example, in Figure 15.2.a, the student is supposed to notice the congruent angles of the isosceles triangle, to observe that the given segments are medians in the triangle, and to move successively inside the small triangles in order to determine the measure of the unknown angles 96 References Ainley, J (1999) Doing Algebra-Type Stuff: emergent algebra in the primary school, in O Zaslavsky (Ed.) Proceedings of the Twenty Third Annual Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education, Haifa, Israel, Vol 2, 9-16 Anderson, K L., Casey, M B., Thompson, W L., Burrage,M S., Pezaris, E., & Kosslyn, S.M (2008) Performance on middle school geometry problems with geometry clues matched to three different cognitive styles Mind, Brain, and Education, 2, 188–197 Anderson, L.W., Krathwohl, D.R., Airasian, P.W., Cruikshank, K.A., Mayer, R.E., Pintrich, P.R., Raths, J., & Wittrock, M.C (2001) A taxonomy for learning, teaching, and assessing: A revision of Bloom's taxonomy of educational objectives New York: Longman Bailin, S., Case, R., Coombs, J R., & Daniels, L B (1999) Common misconceptions of critical thinking Journal of Curriculum Studies, 31(3), 269-283 Baillargeon, R., A Needham, and J DeVos (1992) The development of young infants' intuitions about support Early Development Parenting, 1, 69-78 Ball, D L., Hill, H H., & Bass, H (2005) Knowing mathematics for teaching: Who knows mathematics well enough to teach third grade, and how can we decide? American Educator, Fall, 14-46 Barrett, H C., and Kurzban, R (2006) Modularity in cognition: Framing the debate Psychological Review, 113, 628-647 Bartha, P (2010) By parallel reasoning: The construction and evaluation of analogical arguments New York, NY: Oxford University Press Behl-Chadha, G (1996) Basic-level and superordinate-like categorical representations in early infancy Cognition, 60, 105-141 Benoit, L., Lehalle, H and Jouen, F (2004) Do young children acquire number words through subitizing or counting? Cognitive Development, 19, 291–307 Blömeke, S., Suhl, U., Kaiser, G (2011) Teacher education effectiveness: Quality and equity of future primary teachers’ mathematics and mathematics pedagogical content knowledge Journal of Teacher Education, 62(2), 154-171 Blömeke,S., Kaiser, G., & Clarke, D.(2015) Preface for the Special Issue on Video-Based Research on Teacher Expertise Int J of Sci and Math Educ (2015) 13:257–266 Bloom, B.S and Krathwohl, D R (1956) Taxonomy of Educational Objectives: The Classification of Educational Goals, by a committee of college and university examiners Handbook I: Cognitive Domain NY: Longmans, Green Bloom, P (1994) Generativity within language and other Cognitive Domains Cognition, 51, 177-189 Bowden, E M & Jung-Beeman, M (2003) Aha! - Insight experience correlates with solution activation in the right hemisphere Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10, 730-737 Bransford, J.D., Brown, A.L and Cocking, R.R (Eds.) (2000) How people learn: Brain, mind, experience and school Washington, D.C.: National Academy Press Breidenbach, D., Dubinsky, E., Hawks, J., & Nichols, D (1992) Development of the process conception of function Educational Studies inMathematics, 23, 247–285 Bringuier, J C (1980) Conversations with Jean Piaget Chicago: University of Chicago Press Brown, D (2014) Students' conceptions as dynamically emergent structures Science & Education 23, 1463-1483 97 Brown, S., & Walter, M (1983/2005) The Art of Problem Posing (3rd ed.) Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Bruer, J T (1997) Education and the brain: a bridge too far Educational Researcher, 26, 1-13 Bruer, J.T (1999) In search of brain-based education Phi Delta Kappan, 180, 648-657 Butterworth, B (1999) The Mathematical Brain London: Macmillan Carey, S (1985) Conceptual change in childhood Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Carey, S (1999) Sources of conceptual change In E K Scholnick, K Nelson, S A Gelman & P Miller (Eds.), Conceptual Development: Piaget’s Legacy Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 293-326 Carey, S (2001), Bridging the gap between cognition and developmental neuroscience: The example of number representation, Cognition, 415-431 Carey, S (2004) Bootstrapping and the origin of concepts Daedalus, 59–68 Carey, S., & Spelke, E S (1994) Domain-specific knowledge and conceptual change In L Hirschfeld & S Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture, pp 169-200 Carraher, D., Schliemann, A.D., & Brizuela, B (2000) Early algebra, early arithmetic: Treating operations as functions Plenary address XXII Meeting of the Psychology of Mathematics Education, North American Chapter, Tucson, AZ, October, 2000 Available in CD Case, R (1992) Neo-piagetian theories of intellectual development In H Beilin, & P B Pufall (Eds.), Piaget’s theory: prospects and possibilities (pp 61–107) Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Case, R (1996) Modeling the process of conceptual change in a continuously evolving hierarchical system Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Dev, 61(1-2), 283-295 Case, R., Okamoto, Y., with Griffin, S., McKeough, A., Bleiker, C., Henderson, B., et al (1996) The role of central conceptual structures in the development of children's thought Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 61(5-6, Serial No 246) Chen, Z., & Siegler, R S (2013) Young children's analogical problem solving: Gaining insights from video displays Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116, 904-913 Chi, M T H., Slotta, J D & de Leeuw, N (1994) From things to processes: A theory of conceptual change for learning science concepts Learning and Instruction, 4, 27-43 Chiappe, D., & MacDonald, K (2005) The evolution of domain-general mechanisms in intelligence and learning Journal of General Psychology, 132, 5-40 Chomsky, N (1980) Rules and Representations New York: Columbia University Press Clark, A (1998) Magic Words: How Language Augments Human Computation In P Carruthers and J Boucher (Eds.), Language and Thought: Interdisciplinary Themes (pp 162-183) Cambridge: Cambridge University Press Clement, J (2008) Creative model construction in scientists and students: The role of imagery, analogy, and mental simulation Dordrecht: Springer Commons, M L., Trudeau, E J., Stein, S A., Richards, and F A., Krause, S R (1998) The existence of developmental stages as shown by the hierarchical complexity of tasks Developmental Review, 8(3), 237-278 Cullen, C., Pelczer, I., Singer, F M., Voica, C (2013) When (and How) Does a Posed Problem Become a Problem? in Lindmeier, A M & Heinze, A (Eds.) 37th PME, Vol Kiel, Germany: PME, pp 353-360 98 Dawson-Tunik, T L., Commons, M., Wilson, M., & Fischer, K W (2005) The shape of development European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 2, 163-195 De Corte, E., Verschaffel, L., Lowyck, J., Dhert, S., & Vandeput, L (2002) Collaborative learning of mathematics: Problem-solving and problem-posing supported by “Knowledge Forum” In D Passey & M Kendall (Eds.), TelE-Learning: The challenge for the third millenium (pp 53-59) Boston/Dordrecht/London: Kluwer Academic Publishers Dehaene, S (1997) The number sense: How the mind creates mathematics New York: Oxford U.P Dehaene, S (2001) Précis of The Number Sense, Mind & Language 16 (1), 16–36 Dehaene, S (2007) A few steps toward a science of mental life Mind, Brain, and Education, 1, 28-47 Dehaene, S., & Marques, J F (2002) Cognitive neuroscience: Scalar variability in price estimation and the cognitive consequences of switching to the euro Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 55, 705–731 Dehaene, S., Piazza, M., Pinel, P., and Cohen, L (2003) Three parietal circuits for number processing Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20 (3/4/5/6), 487–506 diSessa, A (1993) Toward an epistemology of physics Cognition and Instruction, 10, 105-225 Dotan, D., & Dehaene, S (2013) How we convert a number into a finger trajectory? Cognition, 129(3), 512e529 http:// dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.07.007 Duit, R (2009) Bibliography–STCSE Students’ and teachers’ conceptions and science education Duit, R and Treagust, D.F (2003) Conceptual change: a powerful framework for improving science teaching and learning, International journal of science education, 25 (6), 671-688 Einstein, A., & Infeld, L (1938) The evolution of physics New York: Simon & Schuster Eisenhardt, K M., Furr, N R., & Bingham, C B (2010) Microfoundations of performance: Balancing efficiency and flexibility in dynamic environments Organization Science, 21(6), 1263–1273 Ellerton, N F., Singer, F M., & Cai, J (2015) Problem Posing in Mathematics: Reflecting on the Past, Energizing the Present, and Foreshadowing the Future Mathematical Problem Posing: From Research to Effective Practice, NY: Springer, 547-556 Feldman, D H (1980/1994) Beyond universals in cognitive development Norwood, NJ: Ablex Feldman, D H (2004) Piaget’s stages: the unfinished symphony of cognitive development New Ideas in Psychology, 22, 175–231 Fischbein, E (1987) Intuition in science and mathematics Dodrecht, Holland: Reidel Fischbein, E., Deri, M., Nello, M.S and Marino, M.S (1985) The role of implicit models in solving verbal problems in multiplication and division, Journal for Research in Mathematics Education 16 pp 3-17 Fischer, K W & Bidell, T R (2006) Dynamic development of action and thought In W.Damon & R M Lerner (Eds.), Theoretical models of human development Handbook of child psychology (6th ed., Vol 1, pp 313-399) New York: Wiley Fischer, K W (2008) Dynamic cycles of cognitive and brain development: Measuring growth in mind, brain, and education In: A.M Battro, K.W Fischer & P Lena (Eds.), The educated brain (127-150) Cambridge U.K.: Cambridge University Press Fischer, K W., and Rose, S P (1994) Dynamic development of coordination of components in brain and behavior: A framework for theory and research In G Dawson and K W Fischer (Eds.), Human behavior and the developing brain (pp 3-66) New York: Guilford Press 99 Fischer, K W., Yan, Z., and Stewart, J (2003) Adult cognitive development: Dynamics in the developmental web In J Valsiner, and K Connolly (Eds.), Handbook of developmental psychology (pp 491-516) Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Fischer, K.W & Rose, L.T (2001) Webs of skill: How students learn Educational Leadership, 59(3), 612 Fodor, J A (1983) The modularity of mind, Cambridge MA: MIT Press Fodor, J.A (2000) The mind doesn’t work that way: The scope and limits of computational psychology Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Gallese, V., & Lakoff, G (2005) The brain’s concepts: The role of the sensory-motor system in conceptual knowledge Cognitive Neuropsychology, 22(3), 455–479 Gallistel, C., & R Gelman (1992) Preverbal and verbal counting and computation Special issue: Numerical cognition Cognition, 44, 43-74 Gardner, H & Davis, K (2013) The “App” Generation: Identity, Intimacy and Imagination in Today’s Youth Gardner, H (1991) The unschooled mind New York: Basic Books Gardner, H (1999) The disciplined mind New York: Simon & Schuster Gentner, D., Holyoak, K., Kokinov, B., eds (2001) The Analogical Mind: Perspectives from Cognitive Science Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Gentner, D., Loewenstein, J., & Thompson, L (2003) Learning and transfer: A general role for analogical encoding Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 393–408 Gick, M L., & Holyoak, K J (1983) Schema induction and analogical transfer Cognitive Psychology, 15, 1–38 Givvin, K B., Stigler, J W., & Thompson, B J (2011) What community college developmental mathematics students understand about mathematics: II The interviews Mathematic Educator, 2(3), 4–18 Goldin, G A (2002) Representation in mathematical learning and problem solving In L D English (Ed.), Handbook of international research in mathematics education (pp 197- 218) Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers Gopnik, A., & Schulz, L (2004) Mechanisms of theory-formation in young children Trends in Cognitive Science, 8, 371–377 Gopnik, A., & Wellman, H M (1994) The theory In L A Hirschfeld & S A Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp 257–293) New York: Cambridge University Press Granott, N & Parziale, J (Eds.) (2002) Microdevelopment: Transition processes in development and learning Cambridge: Cambridge University Press Granott, N (1994) Microdevelopment of co-construction of knowledge during problem-solving: Puzzled minds, weird creatures, and wuggles Dissertation Abstracts International, 54(10B), 5409 Granott, N (2002) How microdevelopment creates macrodevelopment: Reiterated sequences, backward transitions, and the Zone of Current Development In N Granott & J Parziale (Eds.), Microdevelopment: Transition processes in development and learning (pp 213-243) Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press 100 Granott, N., Fischer, K W., & Parziale, J (2002) Bridging to the unknown: A transition mechanism in learning and problem-solving In N Granott & J Parziale (Eds.), Microdevelopment: Transition processes in development and learning (pp 131-156) Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press Hadamard, J W (1945) Essay on the psychology of invention in the mathematical field Princeton: Princeton University Press Halford, G S (1993) Children’s understanding: The development of mental models Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Hartnett, P., & Gelman, R (1998) Early understandings of numbers: Paths or barriers to the construction of new understandings? Learning and Instruction, 8, 341–374 Hauser, M D., Chomsky, N & Fitch, W T (2002) The faculty of language: what is it, who has it, an how did it evolve? Science, 298, 1569-1579 Haylock, D (1997) Recognizing mathematical creativity in school children International Reviews on Mathematical Education, 29(3), 68-74 Retrieved March 10, 2003, from Horsten, L (2015) Philosophy of Mathematics, The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Spring 2015 Edition), Edward N Zalta (ed.), URL http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/spr2015/entries/philosophymathematics/ Huang, J Y., & Bargh, J A (2008) Peak of desire: Activating the mating goal changes life stage preferences across living kinds Psychological Science, 19, 573−578 Hummel, J E., & Holyoak, K J (2003) A symbolic-connectionist theory of relational inference and generalization Psychological Review, 110, 220–263 Izard V, Streri A, Spelke, E (2014) Toward exact number: young children use one-to-one correspondence to measure set identity but not numerical equality Cognitive Psychology 72: 27-53 Jay, E S., & Perkins, D N (1997) Problem finding: The search for mechanism In M Runco (Ed.), The creativity research handbook (pp 257–293) New Jersey: Hampton Press Jones, G (2003) Testing two cognitive theories of insight Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29(5), 1017 Kaiser, G., & Stender, P (2013) Complex modelling problems in co-operative, self-directed learning environments In G A Stillman, et al (Eds.), Teaching mathematical modelling: Connecting to research and practice, international perspectives on the teaching and learning of mathematical modelling (pp 277–293) Dordrecht: Springer Kaplan, C A., & Simon, H A (1990) In search of insight Cognitive psychology, 22(3), 374-419 Karmiloff-Smith, A (1992) Beyond Modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive science Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Karmiloff-Smith, A (1994) Precis of Beyond Modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive science Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17, 693-707 Katona, G (1940) Organizing and memorizing: Studies in the psychology of learning and teaching New York: Columbia University Press Knoblich, G., Ohlsson, S., Haider, H., & Rhenius, D (1999) Constraint relaxation and chunk decomposition in insight problem solving Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25(6), 1534 König, J., Blömeke, S & Kaiser, G (2015) Early career math teachers’ GPK and skills: teacher education, teaching experience, and working conditions make a difference? Int J of Sci and Math Educ 13, 331–350 101 Kontorovich, I., Koichu, B., Leikin, R., & Berman, A (2012) A framework for handling the complexity of students' mathematical problem posing in small groups The Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 31(1), 149–161 Kuhn, D (1996) Is good thinking scientific thinking? In D Olson & N Torrance (Eds.), Modes of thought: Explorations in culture and cognition (pp 261–281) Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press Kuhn, T S (1962/1996) The Structure of Scientific Revolutions, Chicago: University of Chicago Press Lakatos, I (1963) Proofs and Refutations (I) The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science Vol 14, No 53 pp 1-25 Lakoff, G & Nuñez, R (2000) Where mathematics comes from: How the embodied mind brings mathematics into being New York: Basic Books Lakoff, G (1987) Women, fire and dangerous things: what categories reveal about the mind, Chicago: University of Chicago Press Lakoff, G., and Johnson, M (1999) Philosophy in the Flesh: The Embodied Mind and Its Challenge to Western Thought New York: Basic Books Le Corre, M., Van de Walle, G., Brannon, E.M., & Carey, S (2006) Re-visiting the competence/performance debate in the acquisition of the counting principles Cognitive Psychology, 52, 130–169 Linn, M C., & Eylon, B S (2011) Science learning and instruction: Taking advantage of technology to promote knowledge integration New York: Routledge Loewenstein, J., Thompson, L., & Gentner, D (2003) Analogical learning in negotiation teams: Comparing cases promotes learning and transfer Academy of Management Learning and Education, 2, 119–127 MacGregor, J N., & Cunningham, J B (2009) The effects of number and level of restructuring in insight problem solving The Journal of Problem Solving, 2(2), Mandler, G & Shebo, B.J (1982) Subitizing: An analysis of its component processes Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 11, 1-22 Margolis, H (1993) Paradigms and barriers How habits of mind govern scientific belief?, The University of Chicago Press, Chicago and London Marques, J F., & Dehaene, S (2004) Developing intuition for prices in euros: Rescaling or relearning prices? Journal of ExperimentalPsychology: Applied, 10, 148–155 Nesher, P (1987) Towards an instructional theory: The role of students' misconceptions For the Learning of Mathematics, 7(3), 33-40 Nesher, P., & Peled, I (1986) Shifts in reasoning Educational Studies in Mathematics, 17, 67-79 Ohlsson, S (1984) Induced strategy shifts in spatial reasoning Acta Psychologica, 57(1), 46-67 Öllinger, M., Jones, G., & Knoblich, G (2006) Heuristics and representational change in two-move matchstick arithmetic tasks Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 2(4), 239-253 Opfer, J E., & Bulloch, M J (2007) Causal relations drive young children's induction, naming, and categorization Cognition, 105, 207-217 Opfer, J E., & DeVries, J M (2008) Representational change and magnitude estimation: Why young children can make more accurate salary comparisons than adults Cognition, 108, 843-849 Opfer, J E., & Siegler, R S (2007) Representational change and children's numerical estimation Cognitive Psychology, 55, 169-195 doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2006.09.002 102 Palfrey, J and Gasser, U (2008) Born Digital: Understanding the First Generation of Digital Natives, NY: Basic Books Pelczer, I., Singer, F M., & Voica, C (2011) In-Service and Pre-Service Teachers’ Strategies of Task Adaptation In Ubuz, B (Ed.), Developing mathematical thinking Proceedings of the 35th Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education, vol 3, pp 369376 Ankara, Turkey: PME ISBN: 978-975-429-262-6 Pelczer, I., Singer, F M., & Voica, C (2013) Cognitive Framing: A case in Problem Posing Procedia – Social and Behavioral Sciences, Special issue: PSIWORLD 2012, Vol 78, 13 May 2013, 195–199 Pelczer, I., Singer, F M., & Voica, C (2014) Improving problem-posing capacities through inservice teacher training programs: challenges and limits, In Liljedahl, P., Oesterle, S., Nicol, C., & Allan, D (Eds.) Proceedings of the Joint Meeting - 401 of PME 38 and PME-NA 36,Vol 4, pp 401-408 Vancouver, Canada: PME 401- 408 Pelczer, I., Singer, F M., & Voica, C (2015) When communication Tasks Become Tools to Enhance Learning Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences, 187, 503-508 Perner, J., Rendl, B., & Garnham, A (2007) Objects of desire, thought, and reality: Problems of anchoring discourse referents in development Mind & Language, 22, 475–513 Petitto, L A (1993) Modularity and constraints in early lexical acquisition: evidence from children's early language and gesture In: P Bloom (Ed.), Language Acquisition: Core Readings, NY: Harvester Petitto, L A., Zatorre, R J., Gauna, K., Nikelski, E J., Dostie, D., and Evans, A C (2000) Speech-like cerebral activity in profoundly deaf people processing signed languages: implications for the neural basis of human language Science, PNAS, 97(25), 13961-13966 Piaget, J (1952) The origins of intelligence in children New York: International University Press (Original work published 1936) Piaget, J (1954) The construction of reality in the child New York: Basic Books Piaget, J (1968/1970) Structuralism New York: Basic Books Piaget, J (1972) Intellectual evolution from adolescence to adulthood Human Development, 15, 1–12 Piaget, J (1976) Symbolic play In J S Bruner, A Jolly, and K Silva (Eds.), Play - Its role in development and evolution New York: Basic Books Pinker, S (1997) How the mind works New York: Norton Popper, K.R (1962) Conjectures and Refutations The Growth of Scientific Knowledge, New York/ London: Basic Books Quinn, P.C., Eimas, P D., and Tarr, M J (2001) Perceptual categorization of cat and dog silhouettes by 3- to 4-month-old infants J Exp Child Psychol, 79, 78-94 Quinn, P.C., Eimas, P.D (1996) Perceptual cues that permit categorical differentiation of animal species by infants Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 63,189-211 Quinn, P.C., Eimas, P.D and Rosenkrantz, S.L (1993) Evidence for representations of perceptually similar natural categories by 3-month-old and 4-month-old infants, Perception 22: 463-475 Resnick, L B., Nesher, P., Leonard, F., Magone, M., Omanson, S., & Peled, I (1989) Conceptual basis of arithmetic errors: The case of decimal fractions Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 20, 8-27 Richland, L E., Stigler, J W., & Holyoak, K J (2012) Teaching the Conceptual Structure of Mathematics, Educational Psychologist, 47:3, 189-203 103 Sansavini, A., J Bertoncini, and G Giovanelli (1997) Newborns discriminate the rhythm of multisyllabic stressed words Developmental Psychology, 33, 3-11 Sasanguie, D., Göbel, S., Moll, K., Smets, K., Reynvoet, B (2013) Approximate number sense, symbolic number processing, or number-space mappings: What underlies mathematics achievement? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114 (3), 418-431 Schauble, L (1996) The development of scientific reasoning in knowledge-rich contexts Developmental Psychology, 32, 102–119 Scheerer, M (1963) Problem-solving Scientific American, 208, 118-128 Shulman, L S (1986) Those who understand: Knowledge growth in teaching Educational Researcher, 15(2), 4–14 Shulman, L S., & Sherin, M G (2004) Fostering communities of teachers as learners: Disciplinary perspectives Journal of Curriculum Studies, 36(2), 135-140 Siegler, R S., Thompson, C A., & Schneider, M (2011) An integrated theory of whole number and fractions development Cognitive psychology, 62(4), 273-296 Siegler, R.S (1996) Emerging minds: The process of change in children’s thinking New York: Oxford University Press Siegler, R.S (2000) The rebirth of children's learning Child Development, 71, 26-35 Singer, F M (2001) Structuring the information – a new way of perceiving the content of learning, ZDM/ MATHDI, 6, 204-217 Singer, F M (2002) Developing mental abilities through structured teaching methodology, in: A Rogerson (Ed.), The Humanistic Renaissance in Mathematics Education, 353-357 Singer, F M (2003) From cognitive science to school practice: building the bridge In N.A Pateman, B.J Dougherty & J Zilliox (Eds.), 27th PME Proceedings, Hawaii 4, 207-214 Singer, F M (2004) The dynamic structural learning: from theory to practice In M Niss & M Blomhoj (Eds.) ICME 10 Proceedings, Regular lecture (1-17) Copenhagen, Denmark: IMFUFA Singer, F M (2004-2006) Textbooks for primary mathematics Grades 1- Bucharest: Sigma Singer, F M (2007a) Beyond Conceptual Change: Using Representations to Integrate Domain-Specific Structural Models in Learning Mathematics Mind, Brain, and Education, 1(2), 84-97 Singer, F M (2007b) Modelling both complexity and abstraction: a paradox? In: W Blum, P Galbraith, H.W Henn & N Mogens (Eds.), Applications and Modelling in Mathematics Education, (233-240) NY: Springer Singer, F M (2009) The Dynamic Infrastructure of Mind - a Hypothesis and Some of its Applications New Ideas in Psychology, 27, 48-74 Singer, F M (2010) Children’s Cognitive Constructions: From Random Trials to Structures, in Jared A Jaworski (Ed.), Advances in Sociology Research, vol 6, pp: 1-35, ISBN: 978-1-60741-879-5 Singer, F M (2012a) Boosting the Young Learners' Creativity: Representational Change as a Tool to Promote Individual Talents (Plenary lecture) In The 7th International Group for Mathematical Creativity and Giftedness (MCG) International Conference Proceedings Busan, South Korea: MCG, p.3-26 Singer, F M (2012b) Exploring mathematical thinking and mathematical creativity through problem posing In Roza Leikin, Boris Koichu, Avi Berman (eds.) Proceedings of International Workshop of Israel Science Foundation on Exploring and advancing mathematical abilities in high achievers University of Haifa, p 119-124 (9-11 Oct, 2012) 104 Singer, F M & Moscovici, H (2008) Teaching and learning cycles in a constructivist approach to instruction Teaching and Teacher Education, 24(6), 1613-1634 Singer, F M & Radu, N (1994, 1995, 1996, 1997) Teacher’s guide in mathematics – grades: 1, 2, 3, (in Romanian and Russian) Bucharest: Sigma Singer, F M & Sarivan, L (2009) Curriculum Reframed MI and New Routes to Teaching and Learning in Romanian Universities In J.Q Chen, S Moran, H Gardner (eds.), Multiple intelligences around the world Pp 230-244 Singer, F M & Sarivan, L (2011) Masterprof: A program to educate Teachers for the Knowledge Society In F.M Singer & L Sarivan (Eds.) Procedia – Social and Behavioral Sciences, 11 (2011), p 7-11 Singer, F M & Voica, C (2003) Perception of infinity in school: does it really help in problem solving? In A Rogerson (Ed.) The Decidable and the Undecidable in Mathematics Education, (252-256) Brno: The Mathematics Education into the 21st Century Project Singer, F M & Voica, C (2007) Children’s perceptions on infinity: Could they be structured? In D Pitta-Pantazi & G Philippou (Eds.) Proceedings of the 5rd Conference of the European Society for Research in Mathematics Education (CERME 5), Larnaca: Univ of Cyprus, TG3, p 506-515 Singer, F M & Voica, C (2008a) Between perception and intuition: thinking about infinity Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 27, 188-205 Singer, F M & Voica, C (2008b) Extrapolating Rules: How Do Children Develop Sequences? In O Figueras (Ed.), 32th PME Proceedings, Morelia, Michoacán, Mexico, vol 4, 256-263 Singer, F M & Voica, C (2009) When the infinite sets uncover structures: an analysis of students’ reasoning on infinity In Tzekaki, M., Kaldrimidou, M & Sakonidis, C (eds.) In Search for Theories in Mathematics Education Proceedings of the 33rd Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education, vol 5, pp 121-128 Thessaloniki, Greece: PME Singer, F M & Voica, C (2010) In Search of Structures: How Does the Mind Explore Infinity? Mind, Brain and Education, 4(2), p 81-93, ISSN: 1751-2271 Singer, F M & Voica, C (2013) A problem-solving conceptual framework and its implications in designing problem-posing tasks Educational Studies in Mathematics 83(1), 9-26 Singer, F M & Voica, C (2015) Is Problem Posing a Tool for Identifying and Developing Mathematical Creativity? Mathematical Problem Posing: From Research to Effective Practice, NY: Springer, 141174 Singer, F M., & Sarivan, L (2006, Eds) Quo vadis, Academia? Reference Points for a Comprehensive Reform in Higher Education, Bucharest: Sigma, 412 p Singer, F M., Ellerton, N F, & Cai, J (2013) Problem-Posing Research in Mathematics Education: New Questions and Directions Educational Studies in Mathematics 83(1), 1-7 Singer, F M., Ellerton, N F & Cai, J (2015) Mathematical Problem Posing Today: A Cross-Cultural View Mathematical Problem Posing: From Research to Effective Practice, NY: Springer, vii-x Singer, F M., Ellerton, N F., & Cai, J (2015, Eds.) Mathematical Problem Posing: From Research to Effective Practice, NY: Springer (570 p) Singer, F M., Ellerton, N., Cai, J (2013, Eds) Problem Posing in Mathematics Teaching and Learning: Establishing a Framework for Research, Educational Studies in Mathematics, Volume 83, Issue (2013), ISSN: 0013-1954 (Print) 1573-0816 (Online) 105 Singer, F M., Ellerton, N., Cai, J., & Leung, E (2011) Problem Posing in Mathematics Learning and Teaching: a Research Agenda In Ubuz, B (Ed.), Developing mathematical thinking Proceedings of the 35th PME, vol 1, pp.137-166 (Research forum) Ankara, Turkey: PME Singer, F M., Pelczer, I., & Voica, C (2011) Problem posing and modification as a criterion of mathematical creativity In Marta Pytlak, Tim Rowland, Ewa Swoboda (Eds) Proceedings of the 3rd Conference of the European Society for Research in Mathematics Education (CERME 7) University of Rzeszów, Poland, 9-13 February, 2011 ISBN 978-83-7338-683-9 Singer, F M., Pelczer, I., & Voica, C (2015) Problem Posing: Students Between Driven Creativity And Mathematical Failure In Proceedings of the 9th Conference of the European Society for Research in Mathematics Education (CERME 9) 4-8 February, 2015 Skemp, R R (1976) Relational understanding and instrumental understanding Mathematics Teaching, 77, 20–26 Smith, C L., Solomon, G., & Carey S (2005) Never getting to zero: Elementary school students’ understanding of the infinite divisibility of number and matter, Cognitive Psychology, 51, 101–140 Smith, J P., diSessa, A A., & Roschelle, J (1993) Misconceptions reconceived - a constructivist analysis of knowledge in transition Journal of Learning Sciences, 3(2), 115-163 Smith, S.M (1995) Getting into and out of mental ruts: A theory of fixation, incubation, and insight In R.J Sternberg & J.A Davidson (Eds.), The nature of insight (pp 229-251) Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Spelke, E (2003) What Makes Us Smart? Core Knowledge and Natural Language In: D Gentner & S Goldin-Meadow (Eds.), Language in Mind (277-311) Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Spiro, R J., Feltovich, P J., Jacobson, M J., & Coulson, R L (1992) Cognitive flexibility, constructivism, and hypertext: Random access instruction for advanced knowledge acquisition in illstructured domains In T M Duffy & D H Jonassen (Eds.), Constructivism and the Technology of Instruction (pp 57–75) Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Starkey, P (1992) The early development of numerical reasoning Cognition, 43, 93-126 Sternberg, R J., & Davidson, J E (1995) The nature of insight Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Strike, K A., & Posner, G J (1992) A revisionist theory of conceptual change In R A Duschl & R J Hamilton (Eds.), Philosophy of science, cognitive psychology, and educational theory and practice Albany: SUNY Press Thagard, P (2000) Coherence in thought and action Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Torrance, E P (1972) Predictive validity of the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking Journal of Creative Behavior, 6, 236-252 Van Geert, P (1991) A dynamic systems model of cognitive and language growth Psychological Review, 98, 3–53 Varma S., & Schwartz D L (2011) The mental representation of integers: an abstract-to-concrete shift in the understanding of mathematical concepts Cognition 121, 363 Voica, C, & Singer, F M (2011a) Creative contexts as ways to strengthen mathematics learning In M Anitei, M Chraif and C Vasile (Eds.), Proceeding on PSIWORLD 2011, Procedia SBS, Vol 33/2012, 538-542 Voica, C & Singer, F M (2011b) Using small scale projects as tools for changing the teaching paradigm In F M Singer & L Sarivan (Eds.) Procedia – Social and Behavioral Sciences, 11, 2011, 200-204 106 Voica, C & Singer, F M (2012) Problem modification as an indicator of deep understanding Proceedings of ICME 12, pp 1533-1542, Seoul, Korea 8-15 July, 2012 Voica, C & Singer, F M (2013) Problem Modification as a Tool for Detecting Cognitive Flexibility in School Children DOI:10.1007/s11858-013-0492-8 Zentralblatt fur didaktik der Mathematik, ISSN: 1863-9690 (Print) 1863-9704 (Online), 45(2), 267-279 Voica, C , Singer, F M (2014) Problem Posing: A Pathway to Identifying Gifted Students, 119-124 Proceedings of The 8th Conference of the International Group for Mathematical Creativity and Giftedness (MCG), 28-30 of July, 2014, University of Denver, Colorado, USA Vosniadou, S (1994) Capturing and modeling the process of conceptual change Learning & Instruction, 4, 45–69 Vosniadou, S (2013) Conceptual change in learning and instruction: The framework theory approach In S Vosniadou (Ed.), International handbook of research on conceptual change (pp 11–30) NY: Routledge Vosniadou, S., Vamvakoussi, X., & Skopeliti, I (2008) The framework theory approach to the problem of conceptual change In S Vosniadou (Ed.), International handbook of research on conceptual change (pp 3–34) New York: Routledge Vygotsky, L S (1934/1986) Thought and Language Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Vygotsky, L S (1978) Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press Wadsworth, B J (2004) Piaget’s Theory of Cognitive and Affective Development, (5th Ed.) Boston, MA: Pearson Education Waltz, J A., Lau, A., Grewal, S K., & Holyoak, K J (2000) The role of working memory in analogical mapping Memory & Cognition, 28, 1205–1212 Wegner, D M (2002) The illusion of conscious will Cambridge, MA: MIT press Wertheimer, M (1959) Productive thinking New York: Harper & Row Wynn, K (1990) Children’s understanding of counting Cognition, 36, 155-193 Wynn, K (1992) Children’s acquisition of the number words and the counting system Cognitive Psychology, 24, 220–251 Wynn, K (1995) Origins of numerical knowledge Mathematical Cognition 3, 35-60 Yan, Z & Fischer, K.W (2002) Always Under Construction - Dynamic Variations in Adult Cognitive Microdevelopment Human Development; 45, 141-160 Yuan, X., & Sriraman, B (2011) An exploratory study of relationships between students’ creativity and mathematical problem posing abilities – Comparing Chinese and U.S students In B Sriraman, K Lee (Eds.), The Elements of Creativity and Giftedness in Mathematics (pp.5-28) Rotterdam, Netherlands: Sense Publishers Zazkis, R., Liljedahl, P., & Gadowsky, K (2003) Conceptions of function translation: Obstacles, intuitions, and rerouting The Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 22, 435–448 107 Acknowledgements I would like to express my gratitude to my colleagues at the University of Hamburg I would like to thank prof dr Eva Arnold, Dean of the Faculty of Education, for inviting me as a visiting professor at the University of Hamburg This was for me an extremely rich and rewarding learning experience I am grateful to prof dr Gabriele Kaiser, for accepting to accompany me in the complex process of developing the habilitation thesis and procedures I am truly thankful for her right-to-the-point guidance in the conditions of an extremely overloaded program as organizer of the 13th International Congress on Mathematical Education – Hamburg 2016, where more than 3000 researchers confirmed participation I very much appreciated the opportunity to be involved in the vivid exchange of ideas with professors and doctoral students in dr Kaiser’s group I have been honored to be, for a while, part of the team, in a hardworking, highly professional and, in the same time, very friendly community of practice I also wish to thank all the colleagues from the Mathematics Education Department, and especially to prof dr Marianne Nolte for the stimulating discussions we had with various occasions I am grateful to my colleagues from UPG University of Ploiesti for their confidence in my efforts, for their ability to create a healthy and fruitful cooperation in the development of our educational programs, and for the hope that we will succeed to build a competitive doctoral school that can learn a lot from our counterparts from University of Hamburg Finally, I would like to thank my family, who gave me the energy and time to go through the habilitation process in a very dense period of many other duties, my husband Boris for his patience and love, and especially my grandchildren for being a source of motivation and support 108 APPENDIX: Full Papers Full Paper 1: Singer, F.M (2007) Beyond Conceptual Change: Using Representations to Integrate Domain-Specific Structural Models in Learning Mathematics Mind, Brain, and Education, 1(2), pp 84-97 110 Full Paper 2: Singer, F M (2009) The Dynamic Infrastructure of Mind - a Hypothesis and Some of its Applications, New Ideas in Psychology, 27(1), 48–74 124 Full Paper 3: Singer, F M., & Moscovici, H (2008) Teaching and learning cycles in a constructivist approach to instruction Teaching and Teacher Education, Vol 24/6, pp 1613-1634 151 Full Paper 4: Singer, M., Sarivan, L (2009) Curriculum Reframed MI and New Routes to Teaching and Learning in Romanian Universities In J.Q Chen, S Moran, H Gardner (Eds.), Multiple intelligences around the world Pp 230-244 New York: Jossey-Bass 173 Full Paper 5: Singer, F.M (2010) Children’s Cognitive Constructions: From Random Trials to Structures In Jared A Jaworski (Ed.), Advances in Sociology Research, vol 6, 1-35 188 Full Paper 6: Singer, F M (2012) Boosting the Young Learners' Creativity: Representational Change as a Tool to Promote Individual Talents (Plenary lecture) 7th International Mathematical Creativity and Giftedness Conference Proceedings Busan, South Korea: MCG, p.3-26 222 Full Paper 7: Singer, F.M., Voica, C (2008) Between perception and intuition: thinking about infinity, The Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 27, pp 188-205 246 Full Paper 8: Singer, F.M & Voica, C (2013) A problem-solving conceptual framework and its implications in designing problem-posing tasks Educational Studies in Mathematics, 83(1), 9-26 264 Full Paper 9: Singer, F.M & Voica, C (2015) Is Problem Posing a Tool for Identifying and Developing Mathematical Creativity? Mathematical Problem Posing: From Research to Effective Practice, NY: Springer, 141-174 282 Full Paper 10: Singer, F M., Ellerton, N., Cai, J (2013) Problem-Posing Research in Mathematics Education: New Questions and Directions Educational Studies in Mathematics 83(1), 1-7 316 109